0% found this document useful (0 votes)
64 views

Tools For Analyzing Talk Part 3: Morphosyntactic Analysis: Brian Macwhinney Carnegie Mellon University

This document describes tools for morphosyntactic analysis of language transcripts, including: - MOR, a morphological analyzer that tags parts of speech and grammatical categories using rule files to analyze word forms. - POST, a program that corrects errors in MOR's output using a trained model to disambiguate tags. - GRASP, a syntactic dependency parser that identifies grammatical relations between words like subjects and objects using a rule-based approach. The document provides details on how these tools work, how to modify their rule files and grammars, and how to build training corpora to improve their accuracy.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
64 views

Tools For Analyzing Talk Part 3: Morphosyntactic Analysis: Brian Macwhinney Carnegie Mellon University

This document describes tools for morphosyntactic analysis of language transcripts, including: - MOR, a morphological analyzer that tags parts of speech and grammatical categories using rule files to analyze word forms. - POST, a program that corrects errors in MOR's output using a trained model to disambiguate tags. - GRASP, a syntactic dependency parser that identifies grammatical relations between words like subjects and objects using a rule-based approach. The document provides details on how these tools work, how to modify their rule files and grammars, and how to build training corpora to improve their accuracy.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 88

Tools for Analyzing Talk

Part 3: Morphosyntactic Analysis

Brian MacWhinney
Carnegie Mellon University
July 14, 2022

https://doi.org/10.21415/T5B97X

When citing the use of TalkBank and CHILDES facilities, please use this reference to the
last printed version:

MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. 3rd Edition.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

This allows us to systematically track usage of the programs and data through
scholar.google.com.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 2

Tools for Analyzing Talk ...........................................................................................................1


Part 3: Morphosyntactic Analysis ..........................................................................................1
Brian MacWhinney .....................................................................................................................1
1 Introduction .......................................................................................................................4
2 Morphosyntactic Coding ................................................................................................5
2.1 One-to-one correspondence ..............................................................................................5
2.2 Tag Groups and Word Groups ...........................................................................................6
2.3 Words .........................................................................................................................................6
2.4 Part of Speech Codes.............................................................................................................7
2.5 Stems ..........................................................................................................................................9
2.6 Affixes.........................................................................................................................................9
2.7 Clitics ....................................................................................................................................... 10
2.8 Compounds ........................................................................................................................... 10
2.9 Punctuation Marks ............................................................................................................. 11
2.10 Sample Morphological Tagging for English .............................................................. 11
2.11 Ongoing development ....................................................................................................... 14
3 Running the Program Chain....................................................................................... 15
4 Morphological Analysis ............................................................................................... 16
4.1 The Design of MOR ............................................................................................................. 16
4.2 Example Analyses ............................................................................................................... 16
4.3 Exclusions in MOR .............................................................................................................. 17
4.4 Unique Options .................................................................................................................... 17
4.5 Categories and Components of MOR ........................................................................... 18
4.6 MOR Part-of-Speech Categories .................................................................................... 19
4.7 MOR Grammatical Categories ........................................................................................ 23
4.8 Compounds and Complex Forms .................................................................................. 24
4.9 Errors and Replacements ................................................................................................ 25
4.10 Affixes...................................................................................................................................... 25
4.11 Control Features and Output Features ....................................................................... 26
5 Correcting errors ........................................................................................................... 27
5.1 Lexicon Building.................................................................................................................. 29
5.2 Disambiguator Mode ......................................................................................................... 30
6 A Formal Description of the Rule Files .................................................................. 31
6.1 Declarative structure ........................................................................................................ 31
6.2 Pattern-matching symbols .............................................................................................. 31
6.3 Variable notation ................................................................................................................ 32
6.4 Category Information Operators .................................................................................. 32
6.5 Arules ...................................................................................................................................... 33
6.6 Crules ...................................................................................................................................... 35
7 Building new MOR grammars ................................................................................... 37
7.1 minMOR.................................................................................................................................. 37
7.2 Adding affixes....................................................................................................................... 37
7.3 Interactive MOR .................................................................................................................. 38
7.4 Testing .................................................................................................................................... 38
7.5 Building Arules .................................................................................................................... 39
Part 3: Morphosyntax 3

7.6 Building crules ..................................................................................................................... 40


8 MOR for Bilingual Corpora ......................................................................................... 43
9 POST ................................................................................................................................... 45
9.1 POSTLIST ............................................................................................................................... 46
9.2 POSTMODRULES.................................................................................................................. 47
9.3 PREPOST ................................................................................................................................ 47
9.4 POSTMORTEM ...................................................................................................................... 48
9.5 POSTTRAIN ........................................................................................................................... 49
9.6 POSTMOD ............................................................................................................................. 52
9.7 TRNFIX .................................................................................................................................. 52
10 GRASP – Syntactic Dependency Analysis .......................................................... 53
10.1 Grammatical Relations ..................................................................................................... 53
10.2 Predicate-head relations ................................................................................................. 54
10.3 Argument-head relations ................................................................................................ 56
10.4 Extra-clausal elements ..................................................................................................... 58
10.5 Cosmetic relations .............................................................................................................. 58
10.6 MEGRASP................................................................................................................................ 59
11 Building a training corpus ..................................................................................... 60
11.1 ROOT, SUBJ, DET, PUNCT ................................................................................................. 60
Example 1a ................................................................................................................................. 60
11.2 OBJ and OBJ2 ........................................................................................................................ 61
11.3 JCT, NJCT and POBJ ............................................................................................................. 62
11.4 PRED ........................................................................................................................................ 63
11.5 AUX ........................................................................................................................................... 64
11.6 NEG ........................................................................................................................................... 65
11.7 MOD and POSS...................................................................................................................... 65
11.8 CONJ and COORD ................................................................................................................. 66
11.9 ENUM ....................................................................................................................................... 66
11.10 POSTMOD .......................................................................................................................... 68
11.11 COMP, LINK....................................................................................................................... 69
11.12 QUANT and PQ................................................................................................................. 70
11.13 CSUBJ, COBJ, CPOBJ, CPRED ........................................................................................ 71
11.14 CJCT and XJCT .................................................................................................................. 73
11.15 CMOD and XMOD ............................................................................................................ 74
11.16 BEG, BEGP, END, ENDP ................................................................................................. 75
11.17 COM and TAG ................................................................................................................... 76
11.18 SRL, APP............................................................................................................................. 77
11.19 NAME, DATE ..................................................................................................................... 78
11.20 INCROOT, OM ................................................................................................................... 79
12 GRs for other languages .......................................................................................... 80
12.1 Spanish ................................................................................................................................... 80
12.2 Chinese ................................................................................................................................... 80
12.3 Japanese ................................................................................................................................. 82
Part 3: Morphosyntax 4

1 Introduction
This third volume of the TalkBank manuals deals with the use of the programs that
perform automatic computation of the morphosyntactic structure of transcripts in CHAT
format. These manuals, the programs, and the TalkBank datasets can all be downloaded
freely from https://talkbank.org.
The first volume of the TalkBank manual describes the CHAT transcription format.
The second volume describes the use of the CLAN data analysis programs. This third
manual describes the use of the MOR, POST, POSTMORTEM, and MEGRASP
programs to add a %mor and %gra line to CHAT transcripts. The %mor line provides a
complete part-of-speech tagging for every word indicated on the main line of the
transcript. The %gra line provides a further analysis of the grammatical dependencies
between items in the %mor line. These programs for morphosyntactic analysis are all
built into CLAN.
Users who do not wish to create or process information on the %mor and %gra lines
will not need to read this current manual. However, researchers and clinicians interested
in these features will need to know the basics of the use of these programs, as described
in the next chapter. The additional sections of this manual are directed to researchers
who wish to extend or improve the coverage of MOR and GRASP grammars or who
wish to build such grammars for languages that are not yet covered.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 5

2 Morphosyntactic Coding
Linguists and psycholinguists rely on the analysis of morphosyntax to illuminate core
issues in learning and development. Generativist theories have emphasized issues such
as: the role of triggers in the early setting of a parameter for subject omission (Hyams &
Wexler, 1993), evidence for advanced early syntactic competence (Wexler, 1998),
evidence for early absence functional categories that attach to the IP node (Radford,
1990), the role of optional infinitives in normal and disordered acquisition (Rice, 1997),
and the child’s ability to process syntax without any exposure to relevant data (Crain,
1991). Generativists have sometimes been criticized for paying inadequate attention to
the empirical patterns of distribution in children’s productions. However, work by
researchers in this tradition, such as Stromswold (1994), van Kampen (1998), and Meisel
(1986), demonstrates the important role that transcript data can play in evaluating
alternative generative accounts.
Learning theorists have placed an even greater emphasis on the use of transcripts for
understanding morphosyntactic development. Neural network models have shown how
cue validities can determine the sequence of acquisition for both morphological
(MacWhinney & Leinbach, 1991; MacWhinney, Leinbach, Taraban, & McDonald, 1989;
Plunkett & Marchman, 1991) and syntactic (Elman, 1993; Mintz, Newport, & Bever,
2002; Siskind, 1999) development. This work derives further support from a broad
movement within linguistics toward a focus on data-driven models (Bybee & Hopper,
2001) for understanding language learning and structure. These accounts formulate
accounts that view constructions (Tomasello, 2003) and item-based patterns
(MacWhinney, 1975) as the loci for statistical learning.
The study of morphosyntax also plays an important role in the study and treatment of
language disorders, such as aphasia, specific language impairment, stuttering, and
dementia. For this work, both researchers and clinicians can benefit from methods for
achieving accurate automatic analysis of correct and incorrect uses of morphosyntactic
devices. To address these needs, the TalkBank system uses the MOR command to
automatically generate candidate morphological analyses on the %mor tier, the POST
command to disambiguate these analyses, and the MEGRASP command to compute
grammatical dependencies on the %gra tier.

2.1 One-to-one correspondence


MOR creates a %mor tier with a one-to-one correspondence between words on the
main line and words on the %mor tier. In order to achieve this one-to-one
correspondence, the following rules are observed:
1. Each word group (see below) on the %mor line is surrounded by spaces or an initial
tab to correspond to the corresponding space-delimited word group on the main line.
The correspondence matches each %mor word (morphological word) to a main line
word in a left-to-right linear order in the utterance.
2. Utterance delimiters are preserved on the %mor line to facilitate readability and
analysis. These delimiters should be the same as the ones used on the main line.
3. Along with utterance delimiters, the satellite markers of ‡ for the vocative and „ for
tag questions or dislocations are also included on the %mor line in a one-to-one
Part 3: Morphosyntax 6

alignment format.
4. Retracings and repetitions are excluded from this one-to-one mapping, as are
nonwords such as xxx or strings beginning with &. When word repetitions are
marked in the form word [x 3], the material in parentheses is stripped off and the
word is considered as a single form.
5. When a replacing form is indicated on the main line with the form [: text], the
material on the %mor line corresponds to the replacing material in the square
brackets, not the material that is being replaced. For example, if the main line has
gonna [: going to], the %mor line will code going to.
6. The [*] symbol that is used on the main line to indicate errors is not duplicated on
the %mor line.

2.2 Tag Groups and Word Groups


On the %mor line, alternative taggings of a given word are clustered together in tag
groups. These groups include the alternative taggings of a word that are produced by the
MOR program. Alternatives are separated by the ^ character. Here is an example of a tag
group for one of the most ambiguous words in English:
adv|back^adj|back^n|back^v|back

After you run the POST program on your files, all of these alternatives will be
disambiguated and each word will have only one alternative. You can also use the hand
disambiguation method built into the CLAN editor to disambiguate each tag group case
by case.
The next level of organization for the MOR line is the word group. Word groups are
combinations marked by the preclitic delimiter $, the postclitic delimiter ~ or the
compound delimiter +. For example, the Spanish word dámelo can be represented as
vimpsh|da-2S&IMP~pro:clit|1S~pro:clit|OBJ&MASC=give

This word group is a series of three words (verb~postclitic~postclitic) combined by the ~


marker. Clitics may be either preclitics or postclitics. Separable prefixes of the type found
in German or Hungarian and other discontinuous morphemes can be represented as word
groups using the preclitic delimiter $, as in this example for ausgegangen (“gone”):
prep|aus$PART#v|geh&PAST:PART=go

Note the difference between the coding of the preclitic “aus” and the prefix “ge” in this
example. Compounds are also represented as combinations, as in this analysis of
angelfish.
n|+n|angel+n|fish

Here, the first characters (n|) represent the part of speech of the whole compound and the
subsequent tags, after each plus sign, are for the parts of speech of the components of the
compound. Proper nouns are not treated as compounds. Therefore, they take forms with
underlines instead of pluses, such as Luke_Skywalker or New_York_City.

2.3 Words
Beneath the level of the word group is the level of the word. The structure of each
individual word is:
Part 3: Morphosyntax 7

prefix#
part-of-speech|
stem
&fusionalsuffix
-suffix
=english (optional, underscore joins words)

There can be any number of prefixes, fusional suffixes, and suffixes, but there should
be only one stem. Prefixes and suffixes should be given in the order in which they occur
in the word. Since fusional suffixes are fused parts of the stem, their order is
indeterminate. The English translation of the stem is not a part of the morphology, but is
included for convenience for non-native speakers. If the English translation requires two
words, these words should be joined by an underscore as in “lose_flowers” for French
défleurir.
Now let us look in greater detail at the nature of each of these types of coding.
Throughout this discussion, bear in mind that all coding is done on a word-by-word basis,
where words are considered to be strings separated by spaces.

2.4 Part of Speech Codes


The morphological codes on the %mor line begin with a part-of-speech code. The
basic scheme for the part-of-speech code is:
category:subcategory:subcategory

Additional fields can be added, using the colon character as the field separator. The
subcategory fields contain information about syntactic features of the word that are not
marked overtly. For example, you may wish to code the fact that Italian “andare” is an
intransitive verb even though there is no single morpheme that signals intransitivity. You
can do this by using the part-of-speech code v:intrans, rather than by inserting a separate
morpheme.
In order to avoid redundancy, information that is marked by a prefix or suffix is not
incorporated into the part-of-speech code, as this information will be found to the right of
the | delimiter. These codes can be given in either uppercase, as in ADJ, or lowercase, as
in adj. In general, CHAT codes are not case-sensitive.
The particular codes given below are the ones that MOR uses for automatic morpho-
logical tagging of English. Individual researchers will need to define a system of part-of-
speech codes that correctly reflects their own research interests and theoretical commit-
ments. Languages that are typologically quite different from English may have to use
very different part-of-speech categories. Quirk, Greenbaum, Leech, and Svartvik (1985)
explain some of the intricacies of part-of-speech coding. Their analysis should be taken
as definitive for all part-of-speech coding for English. However, for many purposes, a
more coarse-grained coding can be used.
The following set of top-level part-of-speech codes is the one used by the MOR pro-
gram. Additional refinements to this system can be found by studying the organization of
the lexicon files for that program For example, compounds use the main part-of-speech
code, along with codes for their components. Further distinctions can be found by
looking at the MOR lexicon.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 8

English Parts of Speech

Category Code
Adjective adj
Adjective - Predicative adj:pred
Adverb adv
Adverb - Temporal adv:tem
Communicator co
Complementizer comp
Conjunction conj
Coordinator coord
Determiner Article det:art
Determiner - Demonstrative det:dem
Determiner - Interrogative det:int
Determiner - Numeral det:num
Determiner - Possessive det:poss
Filler fil
Infinitive inf
Negative neg
Noun n
Noun - letter n:let
Noun - plurale tantum n:pt
Proper Noun n:prop
Onomatopoeia on
Particle part
Postmodifier post
Preposition prep
Pronoun - demonstrative pro:dem
Pronoun - existential pro:exist
Pronoun - indefinite pro:indef
Pronoun - interrogative pro:int
Pronoun - object pro:obj
Pronoun - personal pro:per
Pronoun - possessive pro:poss
Pronoun - reflexive pro:refl
Pronoun - relative pro:rel
Pronoun - subject pro:sub
Quantifier qn
Verb v
Verb - auxiliary aux
Verb - copula cop
Verb - modal mod
Part 3: Morphosyntax 9

2.5 Stems
Every word on the %mor tier must include a “lemma” or stem as part of the
morpheme analysis. The stem is found on the right hand side of the | delimiter, following
any pre-clitics or prefixes. If the transcript is in English, this can be simply the canonical
form of the word. For nouns, this is the singular. For verbs, it is the infinitive. If the
transcript is in another language, it can be the English translation. A single form should
be selected for each stem. Thus, the English indefinite article is coded as det|a with the
lemma “a” whether or not the actual form of the article is “a” or “an.”

When English is not the main language of the transcript, the transcriber must decide
whether to use English stems. Using English stems has the advantage that it makes the
corpus more available to English-reading researchers. To show how this is done, take the
German phrase “wir essen”:
*FRI: wir essen.
%mor: pro|wir=we v|ess-INF=eat .

Some projects may have reasons to avoid using English stems, even as translations.
In this example, “essen” would be simply v|ess-INF. Other projects may wish to use only
English stems and no target-language stems. Sometimes there are multiple possible trans-
lations into English. For example, German “Sie”/sie” could be either “you,” “she,” or
“they.” Choosing a single English meaning for the stem helps fix the German form.

2.6 Affixes
Affixes and clitics are coded in the position in which they occur with relation to the
stem. The morphological status of the affix should be identified by the following markers
or delimiters: - for a suffix, # for a prefix, and & for fusional or infixed morphology.
The & is used to mark affixes that are not realized in a clearly isolable phonological
shape. For example, the form “men” cannot be broken down into a part corresponding to
the stem “man” and a part corresponding to the plural marker, because one cannot say
that the vowel “e” marks the plural. For this reason, the word is coded as n|man&PL.
The past forms of irregular verbs may undergo similar ablaut processes, as in “came,”
which is coded v|come&PAST, or they may undergo no phonological change at all, as in
“hit”, which is coded v|hit&PAST. Sometimes there may be several codes indicated
with the & after the stem. For example, the form “was” is coded v|be&PAST&13s.
Affix and clitic codes are based either on Latin forms for grammatical function or English
words corresponding to particular closed-class items. MOR uses the following set of affix
codes for automatic morphological tagging of English.

Inflectional Affixes for English

Function Code
adjective suffix er, r CP
adjective suffix est, st SP
noun suffix ie DIM
noun suffix s, es PL
Part 3: Morphosyntax 10

noun suffix 's, ' POSS


verb suffix s, es 3S
verb suffix ed, d PAST
verb suffix ing PRESP
verb suffix en PASTP

Derivational Affixes for English

Function Code
adjective and verb prefix un UN
adverbializer ly LY
nominalizer er ER
noun prefix ex EX
verb prefix dis DIS
verb prefix mis MIS
verb prefix out OUT
verb prefix over OVER
verb prefix pre PRE
verb prefix pro PRO
verb prefix re RE

2.7 Clitics
Clitics are marked by a tilde, as in v|parl&IMP:2S=speak~pro|DAT:MASC:SG for Italian
“parlagli” and pro|it~v|be&3s for English “it's.” Note that part of speech coding with the |
symbol is repeated for clitics after the tilde. Both clitics and contracted elements are
coded with the tilde. The use of the tilde for contracted elements extends to forms like
“sul” in Italian, “ins” in German, or “rajta” in Hungarian in which prepositions are
merged with articles or pronouns.

Clitic Codes for English

Clitic Code
noun phrase post-clitic 'd v:aux|would, v|have&PAST
noun phrase post-clitic 'll v:aux|will
noun phrase post-clitic 'm v|be&1S, v:aux|be&1S
noun phrase post-clitic 're v|be&PRES, v:aux|be&PRES
noun phrase post-clitic 's v|be&3S, v:aux|be&3S
verbal post-clitic n't neg|not

2.8 Compounds
Here are some words that we might want to treat as compounds: sweatshirt,
highschool, playground, and horseback. You can find hundreds of these in files in the
English lexicon such as adv+n+adv.cut that include the plus symbol in their names. There
Part 3: Morphosyntax 11

are also many idiomatic phrases that could be best analyzed as linkages. Here are some
examples: a_lot_of, all_of_a_sudden, at_last, for_sure, kind_of, of_course,
once_and_for_all, once_upon_a_time, so_far, and lots_of. You can find hundreds of
these in files in the English lexicon with names such as adj_under.cut.
On the %mor tier it is necessary to assign a part-of-speech label to each segment of
the compound. For example, the word blackboard is coded on the %mor tier as
n|+adj|black+n|board. The part of speech of the compound as a whole is usually given
by the part-of-speech of the final segment, although this is not always true.
In order to preserve the one-to-one correspondence between words on the main line
and words on the %mor tier, words that are not marked as compounds on the main line
should not be coded as compounds on the %mor tier. For example, if the words “come
here” are used as a rote form, then they should be written as “come_here” on the main
tier. On the %mor tier this will be coded as v|come_here. It makes no sense to code this
as v|come+adv|here, because that analysis would contradict the claim that this pair
functions as a single unit. It is sometimes difficult to assign a part-of-speech code to a
morpheme. In the usual case, the part-of-speech code should be chosen from the same set
of codes used to label single words of the language. For example, some of these idiomatic
phrases can be coded as compounds on the %mor line.

Phrases Coded as Linkages

Phrase Phrase
qn|a_lot_of adv|all_of_a_sudden
co|for_sure adv:int|kind_of
adv|once_and_for_all adv|once_upon_a_time
adv|so_far qn|lots_of.

2.9 Punctuation Marks


MOR can be configured to recognize certain punctuation marks as whole word
characters. In particular, the file punct.cut contains these entries:
„ {[scat end]} "end"
‡ {[scat beg]} "beg"
, {[scat cm]} "cm"

When the punctuation marks on the left occur in text, they are treated as separate lexical
items and are mapped to forms such as beg|beg on the %mor tier. The “end” marker is
used to mark postposed forms such as tags and sentence final particles. The “beg”
marker is used to mark preposed forms such as vocatives and communicators. These
special characters are important for correctly structuring the dependency relations for the
GRASP program.

2.10 Sample Morphological Tagging for English


The following table describes and illustrates a more detailed set of word class
codings for English. The %mor tier examples correspond to the labellings MOR produces
for the words in question. It is possible to augment or simplify this set, either by creating
additional word categories, or by adding additional fields to the part-of-speech label, as
Part 3: Morphosyntax 12

discussed previously. The entries in this table and elsewhere in this manual can always
be double-checked against the current version of the MOR grammar by typing “mor +xi”
to bring up interactive MOR and then entering the word to be analyzed.

Word Classes for English

Class Examples Coding of Examples


adjective big adj|big
adjective, comparative bigger, better adj|big-CP, adj|good&CP
adjective, superlative biggest, best adj|big-SP, adj|good&SP
adverb well adv|well
adverb, ending in ly quickly adv:adj|quick-LY
adverb, intensifying very, rather adv:int|very, adv:int|rather
adverb, post-qualifying enough, indeed adv|enough, adv|indeed
adverb, locative here, then adv:loc|here, adv:tem|then
communicator aha co|aha
conjunction, coord. and, or conj:coo|and, conj:coo|or
conjunction, subord. if, although conj:sub|if, conj:sub|although
determiner, singular a, the, this det|a, det|this
determiner, plural these, those det|these, det|those
determiner, possessive my, your, her det:poss|my
infinitive marker to inf|to
noun, common cat, coffee n|cat, n|coffee
noun, plural cats n|cat-PL
noun, possessive cat's n|cat~poss|s
noun, plural possessive cats' n|cat-PL~poss|s
noun, proper Mary n:prop|Mary
noun, proper, plural Mary-s n:prop|Mary-PL
noun, proper, possessive Mary's n:prop|Mary~poss|s
noun, proper, pl. poss. Marys' n:prop|Mary-PL~poss|s
noun, adverbial home, west n|home, adv:loc |home
number, cardinal two det:num|two
number, ordinal second adj|second
postquantifier all, both post|all, post|both
preposition in prep|in, adv:loc|in
pronoun, personal I, me, we, us, he pro|I, pro|me, pro|we, pro|us
pronoun, reflexive myself, ourselves pro:refl|myself
pronoun, possessive mine, yours, his pro:poss|mine, pro:poss:det|his
pronoun, demonstrative that, this, these pro:dem|that
pronoun, indefinite everybody, nothing pro:indef|everybody
pronoun, indef., poss. everybody's pro:indef|everybody~poss|s
quantifier half, all qn|half, qn|all
verb, base form walk, run v|walk, v|run
verb, 3rd singular present walks, runs v|walk-3S, v|run-3S
Part 3: Morphosyntax 13

verb, past tense walked, ran v|walk-PAST, v|run&PAST


verb, present participle walking, running part|walk-PRESP, part|run-PRESP
verb, past participle walked, run part|walk-PASTP, part|run&PASTP
verb, modal auxiliary can, could, must aux|can, aux|could, aux|must

Since it is sometimes difficult to decide what part of speech a word belongs to, we
offer the following overview of the major part-of-speech labels used in the standard
English grammar.

ADJectives modify nouns, either prenominally, or predicatively. Unitary compound


modifiers such as good-looking should be labeled as adjectives.

ADVerbs cover a heterogenous class of words including: manner adverbs, which


generally end in -ly; locative adverbs, which include expressions of time and place;
intensifiers that modify adjectives; and post-head modifiers, such as indeed and
enough.

COmmunicators are used for interactive and communicative forms which fulfill a
variety of functions in speech and conversation. Also included in this category are
words used to express emotion, as well as imitative and onomatopeic forms, such as
ah, aw, boom, boom-boom, icky, wow, yuck, and yummy.

CONJunctions conjoin two or more words, phrases, or sentences. Examples include:


although, because, if, unless, and until.

COORDinators include and, or, and as well as. These can combine clauses, phrases, or
words.

DETerminers include articles, and definite and indefinite determiners. Possessive deter-
miners such as my and your are tagged det:poss.

INFinitive is the word “to” which is tagged inf|to.

INTerjections are similar to communicators, but they typically can stand alone as
complete utterances or fragments, rather than being integrated as parts of the utterances.
They include forms such as wow, hello, good-morning, good-bye, please, thank-you.

Nouns are tagged with n for common nouns, and n:prop for proper nouns (names of
people, places, fictional characters, brand-name products).

NEGative is the word “not” which is tagged neg|not.

NUMbers are labelled num for cardinal numbers. The ordinal numbers are adjectives.

Onomatopoeia are words that imitate the sounds of nature, animals, and other noises.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 14

Particles are words that are often also prepositions, but are serving as verbal particles.

PREPositions are the heads of prepositional phrases. When a preposition is not a part of
a phrase, it should be coded as a particle or an adverb.

PROnouns include a variety of structures, such as reflexives, possessives, personal


pronouns, deictic pronouns, etc.

QUANTifiers include each, every, all, some, and similar items.

Verbs can be either main verbs, copulas, or auxililaries.

2.11 Ongoing development


Currently, the most highly developed MOR grammar is the one for English, which
achieves 99.18% accuracy in tagging for productions from adult native speakers in
databases such as CHILDES and AphasiaBank. It is more difficult to reliably determine
the accuracy of tagging for child utterances, particularly at the youngest ages when there
are often ambiguities in one-word and two-word utterances (Bloom, 1973) that even
human coders cannot resolve. MOR grammars are also highly evolved for Spanish,
French, German, Mandarin, Japanese, and Cantonese, achieving over 95% accuracy for
these languages too. Apart from accuracy of tagging, there is the issue of lexical
coverage. For child language, lexical coverage is largely complete for these languages.
However, as we deal with more advanced adult productions, such as the academic
language in the MICASE corpus or the juridicial language in the SCOTUS corpus, we
often need to add new items to the lexicons for given language. Accuracy of grammatical
relation tagging by GRASP is highly dependent on accurate tagging by MOR. However,
even with accurate MOR tagging, GRASP tagging is at 93% accuracy for English.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 15

3 Running the Program Chain


It is possible to construct a complete automatic morphosyntacgtic analysis of a series
of CHAT transcripts through a single command in CLAN, once you have the needed
programs in the correct configuration. This command runs the MOR, POST,
POSTMORTEM, and MEGRASP commands in an automatic sequence or chain. To do
this, you follow these steps:
1. Place all the files you wish to analyze into a single folder.
2. Start the CLAN program (see the Part 2 of the manual for instructions on installing
CLAN).
3. In CLAN’s Commands window, click on the buttom labelled Working to set your
working directory to the folder that has the files to be analyzed.
4. Under the File menu at the top of the screen, select Get MOR Grammar and select
the language you want to analyze. To do this, you must be connected to the
Internet. If you have already done this once, you do not need to do it again. By
default, the MOR grammar you have chosen will download to your desktop.
5. If you choose to move your MOR grammar to another location, you will need to use
the Mor Lib button in the Commands window to tell CLAN about where to locate
it.
6. To analyze all the files in your Working directory folder, enter this command in the
Comands window: mor *.cha
7. CLAN will then run these programs in sequence: MOR, POST, POSMORTEM, and
MEGRASP. These programs will add %mor and %gra lines to your files.
8. If this command ends with a message saying that some words were not recognized,
you will probably want to fix them. If you do not, some of the entries on the %mor
line will be incomplete and the relations on the %gra line will be less accurate. If
you have doubts about the spellings of certain words, you can look in the
0allwords.cdc file this is included in the /lex folder for each language. The words
there are listed in alphabetical order.
9. To correct errors, you can run this command: mor +xb *.cha.. Guidelines for fixing
errors are given in chapter 4 below.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 16

4 Morphological Analysis
4.1 The Design of MOR
The computational design of MOR was guided by Roland Hausser’s (1990) MORPH
system and was implemented by Mitzi Morris. Since 2000, Leonid Spektor has extended
MOR in many ways. Christophe Parisse built POST and POSTTRAIN (Parisse & Le
Normand, 2000). Kenji Sagae built MEGRASP as a part of his dissertation work for the
Language Technologies Institute at Carnegie Mellon University (Sagae, MacWhinney, &
Lavie, 2004a, 2004b). Leonid Spektor then integrated the program into CLAN.
The system has been designed to maximize portability across languages,
extendability of the lexicon and grammar, and compatibility with the CLAN programs.
The basic engine of the parser is language independent. Language-specific information is
stored in separate data files that can be modified by the user. The lexical entries are also
kept in ASCII files and there are several techniques for improving the match of the
lexicon to a corpus. To maximize the complete analysis of regular formations, only stems
are stored in the lexicon and inflected forms appropriate for each stem are compiled at
run time.

4.2 Example Analyses


To give an example of the results of a MOR analysis for English, consider this sentence
from eve15.cha in Roger Brown’s corpus for Eve.
*CHI: oops I spilled it.
%mor: co|oops pro:subj|I v|spill-PAST pro:per|it.

Here, the main line gives the child’s production and the %mor line gives the part of
speech for each word, along with the morphological analysis of affixes, such as the past
tense mark (-PAST) on the verb. The %mor lines in these files were not created by hand.
To produce them, we ran the MOR command, using the MOR grammar for English,
which can be downloaded using the Get MOR Grammar function described in the
previous chapter. The command for running MOR by itself without running the rest of
the chain is: mor +d *.cha. After running MOR, the file looks like this:
*CHI: oops I spilled it .
%mor: co|oops pro:subj|I part|spill-PASTP^v|spill-PAST pro:per|it
.

In the %mor tier, words are labeled by their syntactic category or “scat”, followed by the
pipe separator |, followed then by the stem and affixes. Notice that the word “spilled” is
initially ambiguous between the past tense and participle readings. The two ambiguities
are separated by the ^ character. To resolve such ambiguities, we run a program called
POST. The command is just “post *.cha” After POST has been run, the %mor line will
only have v|spill-PAST.
Using this disambiguated form, we can then run the MEGRASP program to create
the representation given in the %gra line below:
*CHI: oops I spilled it .
%mor: co|oops pro:subj|I v|spill-PAST pro:per|it .
Part 3: Morphosyntax 17

%gra: 1|3|COM 2|3|SUBJ 3|0|ROOT 4|3|OBJ 5|3|PUNCT

In the %gra line, we see that the second word “I” is related to the verb (“spilled”) through
the grammatical relation (GR) of Subject. The fourth word “it” is related to the verb
through the grammatical relation of Object. The verb is the Root and it is related to the
“left wall” or item 0.

4.3 Exclusions in MOR


Because MOR focuses on the analysis of the target utterance, it excludes a variety of non-
words, retraces, and special symbols. Specifically, MOR excludes:
1. Items that start with &
2. Pauses such as (.)
3. Unknown forms marked as xxx, yyy, or www
4. Data associated with these codes: [/?], [/-], [/], [//], and [///].

4.4 Unique Options


+d do not run POST command automatically. POST will run automatically after
MOR, unless this switch is used or unless the folder name includes the word
“train”.

+eS Show the result of the operation of the arules on either a stem S or stems in file
@S. This output will go into a file called debug.cdc in your library directory. An-
other way of achieving this is to use the +d option inside “interactive MOR”

+p use pinyin lexicon format for Chinese

+xi Run MOR in the interactive test mode. You type in one word at a time to the test
prompt and MOR provides the analysis on line. This facility makes the following
commands available in the CLAN Output window:
word - analyze this word
:q quit- exit program
:c print out current set of crules
:d display application of arules.
:l re-load rules and lexicon files
:h help - print this message

If you type in a word, such as “dog” or “perro,” MOR will try to analyze it and
give you its components morphemes. If you change the rules or the lexicon, use :l
to reload and retest. The :c and :d switches will send output to a file called de-
bug.cdc in your library directory.

+xl Run MOR in the lexicon building mode. This mode takes a series of .cha files as
input and outputs a small lexical file with the extension .ulx with entries for all
words not recognized by MOR. This helps in the building of lexicons.

+xb check lexicon mode, include word location in data files


Part 3: Morphosyntax 18

+xa check lexicon for ambiguous entries


+xc check lexicon mode, including capitalized words
+xd check lexicon for compound words conflicting with plain words
+xp check lexicon mode, including words with prosodic symbols
+xy analyze words in lex files

4.5 Categories and Components of MOR


MOR breaks up words into their component parts or morphemes. In a relatively analytic
language like English, many words require no analysis at all. However, even in English,
a word like “coworkers” can be seen to contain four component morphemes, including
the prefix “co”, the stem, the agential suffix, and the plural. For this form, MOR will
produce the analysis: co#n:v|work-AGT-PL. This representation uses the symbols # and
– to separate the four different morphemes. Here, the prefix stands at the beginning of
the analysis, followed by the stem (n|work), and the two suffixes. In general, stems
always have the form of a part of speech category, such as “n” for noun, followed by the
vertical bar and then a statement of the stem’s lexical form.

To understand the functioning of the MOR grammar for English, the best place to begin
is with a tour of the files inside the ENG folder that you can download from the server.
At the top level, you will see these files:
1. ar.cut – These are the rules that generate allomorphic variants from the stems and
affixes in the lexical files.
2. cr.cut – These are the rules that specify the possible combinations of morphemes
going from left to right in a word.
3. debug.cdc – This file holds the complete trace of an analysis of a given word by
MOR. It always holds the results of the most recent analysis. It is mostly useful for
people who are developing new ar.cut or cr.cut files as a way of tracing out or
debugging problems with these rules.
4. docs – This is a folder containing a file of instructions on how to train POST and a
list of tags and categories used in the English grammar.
5. post.db – This is a file used by POST and should be left untouched.
6. ex.cut – This file includes analyses that are being “overgenerated” by MOR and
should simply be filtered out or excluded whenever they occur.
7. lex – This folder contains many files listing the stems and affixes of the language.
We will examine it in greater detail below.
8. sf.cut – This file tells MOR how to deal with words that end with certain special
form markers such as @b for babbling.
When examining these files and others, please note that the exact shapes of the files, the
word listings, and the rules will change over time. We recommend that users glance
through these various files to understand their contents.

The first action of the parser program is to load the ar.cut file. Next the program
reads in the files in your lexicon folder and uses the rules in ar.cut to build the run-time
lexicon. Once the run-time lexicon is loaded, the parser then reads in the cr.cut file.
Additionally, if the +b option is specified, the dr.cut file is also read in. Once the
concatenation rules have been loaded the program is ready to analyze input words. As a
Part 3: Morphosyntax 19

user, you do not need to concern yourself about the run-time lexicon. Your main concern
is about the entries in the lexicon files. For languages that already have a MOR grammar,
the rules in the ar.cut and cr.cut files are only of concern if you wish to have a set of
analyses and labelings that differs from the one given in the chapter on morphosyntactic
coding, or if you are trying to write a new set of grammars for some language.

4.6 MOR Part-of-Speech Categories


The final output of MOR on the %mor line uses two sets of categories: part-of-
speech (POS) names and grammatical categories. To survey the part-of-speech names
for English, we can take a look at the files contained inside the /lex folder. These files
break out the possible words of English into different files for each specific part of
speech or compound structure. Because these distinctions are so important to the correct
transcription of child language and the correct running of MOR, it is worthwhile to
consider the contents of each of these various files. As the following table shows, about
half of these word types involve different part of speech configurations within
compounds. This analysis of compounds into their part of speech components is intended
to further study of the child’s learning of compounds as well as to provide good
information regarding the part of speech of the whole. The name of the compound files
indicates their composition. For example, the name adj+n+adj.cut indicates compounds
with a noun followed by an adjective (n+adj) whose overall function is that of an
adjective. This means that it is treated just as and adjective (adj) by the MOR and
GRASP programs. In English, the part of speech of a compound is usually the same as
that of the last component of the compound. A few additional part of speech (POS)
categories are introduced by the 0affix.cut file. These include: n-cl (noun clitic), v-cl
(verb clitic), part (participle), and n:gerund (gerund). Additional categories on the %mor
line are introduced from the special form marker file called sf.cut. The meanings of these
various special form markers are given in the CHAT manual. Also, the punctuation
codes end, beg, and cm are the POS codes used for the special character marks given in
the punct.cut file.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 20

File (.cut) POS Function Example


0aae mixed African American finna
0affix mixed prefixes and suffixes see list below
0uk mixed terms local to the UK fave, doofer,
0sing mixed Singapore English bochup, dabao
adj-baby adj baby talk adjectives dipsy, yumsy
adj-irr adj irregular adjectives better, furthest
adj-num adj ordinal numerals eleventh
adj-pred adj:pred predicative adjectives abreast, remiss
adj-sci adj scientific terms biogenous
adj-under adj combined adjectives close_by, lovey_dovey
adj adj regular adjectives tall, redundant
adj+adj+adj adj compounds halfhearted, hot_crossed
adj+adj+adj(on) adj compounds super_duper, easy_peasy
adj+adj+n adj compounds all-time
adj+adj+part adj compounds wholehearted
adj+n+adj adj compounds dogeared, stir_crazy
adj+n+part adj compounds bottlefed
adj+v+* adj compounds various
adv-cp adv can be adj or adv fast, hard
adv-ptl adv verbal particles up, out
adv-tem adv temporals tomorrow, tonight
adv-under adv combined adverbs how_about, as_well
adv-wh adv:wh wh term where, why
adv adv regular adverbs ajar, fast, mostly
adv+adj+n adv compounds heavy_duty
adv+n+adv adv compounds piggyback
adv+n+prep adv compounds thumbs_up
adv+n+prep+n adv compounds face_to_face
adv+prep+n adv compounds offstage
co-biling co Cantonese forms wo, wai, la
co-voc co vocatives honey, dear, sir
co-rhymes co rhymes, onomatopoeia cock_a_doodle_doo
co_under co multiword phrases by_jove, gee_whiz
co co regular co blah, byebye, gah
comp comp complementizers that, which
conj-under conj combined conj even_though, in_case_that
conj conj conjunctions and, although, because
coord coord coordinators and, neither, or
det-art det:art deictic determiners a, the
det-dem det:dem demonstratives this, that
det-int det:int interrogatives what, whose
det-num det:num cardinals two, twelve
det-poss det:poss possessives his, their
Part 3: Morphosyntax 21

n-abbrev n abbreviations c_d, t_v, w_c


n-baby n babytalk forms passie, wawa, booboo
n-dashed n noun combinations cul_de_sac, seven_up
n-dup n varying doubles bibble_wibble, sucky_suck
n-hyphen n hyphenated pre-op, cul-de-sac
n-irr n irregular nouns children, cacti, teeth
n-let n all but a and i c, k, q
n-pluraletant n:pt nouns with no singular golashes, kinesics, scissors
n-sci n scientific terms ampicillin
n-under n not compounds band_aid, beck_and_call
n n regular nouns dog, corner, window
n+adj+n n compounds big+shot, cutie+pie
n+n+conj+n n compounds four+by+four, dot+to+dot
n+n+n-on n compounds quack+duck, moo+cow
n+n+n n compounds candy+bar, foot+race
n+n+novel n compounds children+bed, dog+fish
n+n+part n compounds homemaking
n+n+prep+det+n n compounds corn_on_the_cob
n+on+on-baby n compounds wee+wee, meow+meow
n+other.cut n compounts hide+and+seek
n+v+det+n n compounds follow+the+leader
n+v+n n compounds squirm+worm, snap+bead
n+v+prep n compounds chin_up, hide+out
nonce.cut various jabberwocky mimsy, wabe
n-verb.cdc n usually verbs find, run
on on onomatopoeia boom, choo_choo
on+on+on on compounds cluck+cluck, knock+knock
post post post-modifiers all, too
prep-under prep combined prepositions out_of, in_between
prep prep prepositions under, minus
pro-dem pro:dem demonstratives this, that
pro-exist pro:exist existentials here, there
pro-indef pro:indef indefinites everybody, few
pro-int pro:int interrogative what, who
pro-obj pro:obj object case him, me
pro-per pro:per case neutral it, you
pro-poss pro:poss possessives hers, mine
pro-refl pro:refl reflexives myself, himself
pro-sub pro:sub subject case she, they
punct end, begin special marks ‡ and „
quan qn quantifier some, all, only, most
rel rel relativizers that, which
small inf, neg small forms not, to, xxx, yyy
v-aux aux auxiliaries had, getting
Part 3: Morphosyntax 22

v-baby v baby verbs wee, poo


v-clit v cliticized forms gonna, looka
v-cop cop copula be, become
v-hyphen v hyphenated forms x-ray, ad-lib
v-irr v irregular verbs came, beset, slept
v-mod-aux mod modal auxiliaries hafta, gotta
v-mod-inf mod, v, part cliticized with inf hafta, gonna
v-mod mod modals can, ought
v-sci v scientific forms protonate
v v regular verbs run, take, remember
v+adj+v v compounds deep-fry, tippytoe
v+n+v v compounds bunny+hop, sleepwalk
v-noun v usually nouns father, shoe
zero 0x omitted words 0adj, 0n, 0subj

The construction of these lexicon files involves a variety of decisions. Here are some of
the most important issues to consider.
1. Words may often appear in several files. For example, virtually every noun in
English can also function as a verb. However, when this function is indicated by a
suffix, as in “milking” the noun can be recognized as a verb through a process of
morphological derivation contained in a rule in the cr.cut file. In such cases, it is
not necessary to list the word as a verb. Of course, this process fails for unmarked
verbs. However, it is generally not a good idea to represent all nouns as verbs,
since this tends to overgenerate ambiguity. Instead, it is possible to use the
POSTMORTEM program to detect cases where nouns are functioning as bare
verbs.
2. If a word can be analyzed morphologically, it should not be given a full listing.
For example, since “coworker” can be analyzed by MOR into three morphemes as
co#n:v|work-AGT, it should not be separately listed in the n.cut file. If it is, then
POST will not be able to distinguish co#n:v|work-AGT from n|coworker.
3. The zero-cut file lists possible omitted parts of speech, such as 0det. However, in
the transcript, this could be represented as either 0det or 0the.
4. It is always best to use spaces to break up word sequences that are just
combinations of words. For example, instead of transcribing 1964 as
“nineteen+sixty+four”, “nineteen-sixty-four”, or “nineteen_sixty_four”, it is best
to transcribe simply as “nineteen sixty four”. This principle is particularly
important for Chinese, where there is a tendency to underutilize spaces, since
Chinese is written without spaces.
5. For most languages that use Roman characters, you can rely on capitalization to
force MOR to treat words as proper nouns. To understand this, take a look at the
forms in the sf.cut file at the top of the MOR directory. These various entries tell
MOR how to process forms like k@l for the letter “k” or John_Paul_Jones for the
famous admiral. The symbol \c indicates that a form is capitalized and the symbol
\l indicates that it is lowercase.
6. The pro:int category is used to cover both initial wh-words in utterances such as
why are you smiling, as well as headless clauses such as I know why you are
Part 3: Morphosyntax 23

smiling.

4.7 MOR Grammatical Categories


In addition to the various part-of-speech categories provided by the lexicon, MOR also
inserts a series of grammatical categories, based on the information about affixes in the
0affix.cut file, as well as information inserted by the a-rules and c-rules. If the category
is regularly attached, it is preceded by a dash. If it is irregular, it uses an amerpsand. For
English, the inflectional categories are:

Abbreviation Meaning Example Analysis


PL nominal plural cats n|cat-PL
PAST past tense pulled v|pull-PAST
PRESP present participle pulling v|pull-PRESP
PASTP past participle broken v|break-PASTP
PRES present am cop|be&1S&PRES
1S first singular am cop|be&1S&PRES
3S third singular present is cop|be&3S&PRES
13S first and third was cop|be&PAST&13S

In addition to these inflectional categories, English uses these derivational morphemes:

Abbreviation Meaning Example Analysis


CP comparative stronger adj|strong-CP
SP superlative strongest adj|strong-SP
AGT agent runner n|run&dv-AGT
DIM diminutive doggie n|dog-DIM
FUL denominal hopeful adj|hope&dn-FULL
NESS deadjectival goodness n|good&dadj-NESS
ISH denominal childish adj|child&dn-ISH
ABLE deverbal likeable adj|like&dv-ABLE
LY deadjectival happily adj|happy&dadj-LY
Y deverbal, denominal sticky adj|stick&dn-Y

In these examples, the features dn, dv, and dadj indicate derivation of the forms from
nouns, verbs, or adjectives.

Other languages use many of these same features, but with many additional ones,
particularly for highly inflecting languages. Sometimes these are lowercase and
sometimes upper. Here are some examples:

Affix Meaning Affix Meaning Affix Meaning


KONJ subjunctive ADV adverbial m masculine
SUB subjunctive SG singular f feminine
COND conditional PL plural AUG augmentative
NOM nominative IMP imperative PROG progressive
Part 3: Morphosyntax 24

ACC accusative IMPF imperfective PRET preterite


DAT dative FUT future
GEN genitive PASS passive

4.8 Compounds and Complex Forms


The lexical files include many special compound files such as n+n+n.cut or v+n+v.cut.
Compounds are listed in the lexical files according to both their overall part of speech (X-
bar) and the parts of speech of their components. However, there are seven types of
complex word combinations that should not be treated as compounds.
1. Underscored words. The n-under.cut file includes many forms that resemble
compounds, but which are best viewed as units with non-morphemic components.
For example, kool_aid and band_aid are not analytic combinations of morphemes,
although they clearly have two components. The same is true for hi_fi and
coca_cola. The underscore character can be used as needed when a plus for
compounding is not appropriate. The underscore mark is particularly useful for
representing the combinations of words found in proper nouns such as
John_Paul_Jones, Columbia_University, or The_Beauty_and_the_Beast. If these
words are capitalized, they do not need to be included in the MOR lexicon, since
all capitalized words are taken as proper nouns in English.
2. Separate words. Many noun-noun combinations in English should just be
written out as separate words. An example would be “faucet stem assembly
rubber gasket holder”. It is worth noting here that German treats all such forms as
single words. This means that different conventions must be adopted for German
in order to avoid the need for exhaustive listing of the infinite number of German
compound nouns.
3. Spelling sequences. Sequences of letter names such as “O-U-T” for the spelling
of “out” are transcribed with the suffix @k, as in out@k.
4. Acronyms. Forms such as FBI are transcribed with underscores, as in F_B_I.
Presence of the initial capital letter tells MOR to treat F_B_I as a proper noun.
This same format is used for non-proper abbreviations such as c_d or d_v_d.
5. Products. Coming up with good forms for commercial products such as Coca-
Cola is tricky. Because of the need to ban the use of the dash on the main line, we
have avoided the use of the dash in these names. They should not be treated as
compounds, as in coca+cola, and compounds cannot be capitalized, so Coca+Cola
is not possible. This leaves us with the option of either coca_cola or Coca_Cola.
The option coca_cola seems best, since this is not a proper noun.
6. Babbling and word play. In earlier versions of CHAT and MOR, transcribers
often represent sequences of babbling or word play syllables as compounds. This
was done mostly because the plus provides a nice way of separating out the
separate syllables in these productions. To make it clear that these separations are
simply marked for purposes of syllabification, we now ask transcribers to use
forms such as ba^ba^ga^ga@wp or choo^bung^choo^bung@o to represent these
patterns.
The introduction of this more precise system for transcription of complex forms opens up
additional options for programs like MLU, KWAL, FREQ, and GRASP. For MLU,
Part 3: Morphosyntax 25

compounds will be counted as single words, unless the plus sign is added to the
morpheme delimiter set using the +b+ option switch. For GRASP, processing of
compounds only needs to look at the overall part of speech of the compound, since the
internal composition of the compound is not relevant to the syntax. Additionally, forms
such as "faucet handle valve washer assembly" do not need to be treated as compounds,
since GRASP can learn to treat sequences of nouns as complex phrases header by the
final noun.

4.9 Errors and Replacements


Transcriptions on the main line have to serve two, sometimes conflicting (Edwards,
1992), functions. On the one hand, they need to represent the form of the speech as
actually produced. On the other hand, they need to provide input that can be used for
morphosyntactic analysis. When words are pronounced in their standard form, these two
functions are in alignment. However, when words are pronounced with phonological or
morphological errors, it is important to separate out the actual production from the
morphological target. This can be done through a system for main line tagging of errors.
This system largely replaces the coding of errors on a separate %err line, although that
form is still available, if needed. The form of the newer system is illustrated here:

*CHI: him [* case] ated [: ate] [* +ed-sup] a f(l)ower and a pun


[: bun].

For the first error, there is no need to provide a replacement, since MOR can process
“him” as a standard pronoun. However, since the second word is not a real word form,
the replacement is necessary in order to tell MOR how to process the form. The third
error is just an omission of “l” from the cluster and the final error is a mispronunciation
of the initial consonant. Phonological errors are not coded here, since that level of
analysis is best conducted inside the Phon program (Rose et al., 2005).

4.10 Affixes
The inflectional and derivational affixes of English are listed in the 0affix.cut file.
1. This file begins with a list of prefixes such as “mis” and “semi” that attach either to
nouns or verbs. Each prefix also has a permission feature, such as [allow mis]. This
feature only comes into play when a noun or verb in n.cut or v.cut also has the
feature [pre no]. For example, the verb “test” has the feature [pre no] included in
order to block prefixing with “de-” to produce “detest” which is not a derivational
form of "test". At the same time, we want to permit prefixing with “re-”, the entry
for “test” has [pre no][allow re]. Then, when the relevant rule in cr.cut sees a verb
following “re-” it checks for a match in the [allow] feature and allows the
attachment in this case.
2. Next we see some derivational suffixes such as diminutive –ie or agential –er.
Unlike the prefixes, these suffixes often change the spelling of the stem by dropping
silent e or doubling final consonants. The ar.cut file controls this process, and the
[allo x] features listed there control the selection of the correct form of the suffix.
3. Each suffix is represented by a grammatical category in parentheses. These
Part 3: Morphosyntax 26

categories are taken from a typologically valid list given in the CHAT Manual.
4. Each suffix specifies the grammatical category of the form that will result after its
attachment. For suffixes that change the part of speech, this is given in the scat, as
in [scat adj:n]. Prefixes do not change parts of speech, so they are simply listed as
[scat pfx] and use the [pcat x] feature to specify the shape of the forms to which
they can attach.
5. The long list of suffixes concludes with a list of cliticized auxiliaries and reduced
main verbs. These forms are represented in English as contractions. Many of these
forms are multiply ambiguous and it will be the job of POST to choose the correct
reading from among the various alternatives.

4.11 Control Features and Output Features


The lexical files include several control features that specify how stems should be
treated. One important set includes the [comp x+x] features for compounds. This feature
controls how compounds will be unpacked for formatting on the %mor line. Irregular
adjectives in adj-ir.cut have features specifying their degree as comparative or
superlative. Irregular nouns have features controlling the use of the plural. Irregular
verbs have features controlling consonant doubling [gg +] and the formation of the
perfect tense. Features like [block ed] are used to prevent reocognition of overregularized
forms such as goed.
There are also a variety of features that are included in lexical entries, but not
necessarily present in the final output. For example, the feature of gender is used to
determine patterns of suffixation in Spanish, but to include this feature in the output it
must be present and not commented in the output.cut file. Other lexical features of this
type include root, ptn, num, tense, and deriv.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 27

5 Correcting errors
When running MOR on a new set of CHAT files, it is important to make sure that
MOR will be able to recognize all the words in these files. A first step in this process
involves running the CHECK program to see if all the words follow basic CHAT rules,
such as not including numbers or capital letters in the middle of words. There are several
common reasons for a word not being recognized:
1. It is misspelled. If you have doubts about the spellings of certain words, you can
look in the 0allwords.cdc file this is included in the /lex folder for each language.
The words there are listed in alphabetical order.
2. The word should be preceded by and ampersand & to block look up through MOR.
There are four forms using the ampersand. Nonwords just take the & alone, as in
&gaga. Incomplete words should be transcribed as &+text, as in &+sn for the
beginning of snake. Filler words should be transcribed as &-uh. Finally, sounds like
laughing can be transcribed as &=laughs, as described more extensively in the
CHAT manual.
3. The word should have been transcribed with a special form marker, as in bobo@o or
bo^bo@o for onomatopoeia. It is impossible to list all possible onomatopoeic forms
in the MOR lexicon, so the @o marker solves this problem by telling MOR how to
treat the form. This approach will be needed for other special forms, such as
babbling, word play, and so on.
4. The word was transcribed in “eye-dialect” to represent phonological reductions.
When this is done, there are two basic ways to allow MOR to achieve correct
lookup. If the word can be transcribed with parentheses for the missing material, as
in “(be)cause”, then MOR will be happy. This method is particularly useful in
Spanish and German. Alternatively, if there is a sound substitution, then you can
transcribe using the [: text] replacement method, as in “pittie [: kittie]”.
5. You should treat the word as a proper noun by capitalizing the first letter. This
method works for many languages, but not in German where all nouns are
capitalized and not in Asian languages, since those languages do not have systems
for capitalization.
6. The stem is in the lexicon, but the inflected form is not recognized. In this case, it is
possible that one of the analytic rules of MOR is not working. These problems can
be reported to macw@cmu.edu.
7. The stem or word is missing from MOR. In that case, you can create a file called
something like 0add.cut in the /lex folder of the MOR grammar. Once you have
accumulated a collection of such words, you can email them to macw@cmu.edu for
permanent addition to the lexicon.
Some of these forms can be corrected during the initial process of transcription by
running CHECK. However, others will not be evident until you run the MOR command
with +xb or +xl and get a list of unrecognized words.
To correct these problems, there are basically two possible tools. The first is the
KWAL program built in to CLAN. Let us say that your filename.ulx.cex list of
unrecognized words has the form “cuaght” as a misspelling of “caught.” Let us further
imagine that you have a single collection of 80 files in one folder. To correct this error,
just type this command into the Commands window:
Part 3: Morphosyntax 28

kwal *.cha +scuaght

KWAL will then send input to your screen as it goes through the 80 files. There may
be no more than one case of this misspelling in the whole collection. You will see this as
the output scrolls by. If necessary, just scroll back in the CLAN Output window to find
the error and then triple click to go to the spot of the error and then retype the word
correctly.
For errors that are not too frequent, this method works fairly well. However, if you
have made some error consistently and frequently, you may need stronger methods.
Perhaps you transcribed “byebye” as “bye+bye” as many as 60 times. In this case, you
could use the CHSTRING program to fix this, but a better method would involve the use
of a Programmer’s Editor system such as BBEdit for the Mac or Epsilon for Windows.
Any system you use must include an ability to process Regular Expressions (RegExp)
and to operate smoothly across whole directories at a time. However, let me give a word
of warning about the use of more powerful editors. When using these systems,
particularly at first, you may make some mistakes. Always make sure that you keep a
backup copy of your entire folder before each major replacement command that you
issue.
Once you know that a corpus passes CHECK, you will want to see whether it
contains words that are either misspelled or not yet in the MOR lexicon. You do this by
running the command:
mor +xb *.cha

The output from this command will have the extension .ulx.cex. After running the
command, its name will appear at the end of the output in the CLAN Output window. If
that window tells you that “all words were found in the lexicon”, then you can proceed
with running
mor *.cha

However, if not all words are recognized, you can triple-click on the line listing ther
“Output File” and it will open the list of words not yet recognized by MOR. In any large
corpus, is extremely unlikely that every word would be listed in even the largest MOR
lexicon. Therefore, users of MOR need to understand how to supplement the basic
lexicons with additional entries. Before we look at the process of adding new words to
the lexicon, we first need to examine the way in which entries in the disk lexicon are
structured.
The disk lexicon contains irregular forms of a word as well as the stems of regular
forms. For example, the verb “go” is stored in the disk lexicon, along with the past tense
“went,” since this latter form is suppletive and does not undergo regular rules. The disk
lexicon contains a series of files each with a series of lexical entries with one entry per
line. The lexicon may be annotated with comments, which will not be processed. A
comment begins with the percent sign and ends with a new line. A lexical entry consists
of these parts:
1. The surface form of the word.
2. Category information about the word, expressed as a set of feature-value pairs. Each
feature-value pair is enclosed in square brackets and the full set of feature-value
pairs is enclosed in curly braces. All entries must contain a feature-value pair that
identifies the syntactic category to which the word belongs, consisting of the feature
Part 3: Morphosyntax 29

“scat” with an appropriate value.


3. Following the category information is information about the lemmatization of ir-
regular forms. This information is given by having the citation form of the stem
followed by the & symbol as the morpheme separator and then the grammatical
morphemes it contains.
4. Finally, if the grammar is for a language other than English, you can enter the
English translation of the word preceded by and followed by the = sign.

The following are examples of lexical entries:


can {[scat v:aux]}
a {[scat det]}
an {[scat det]} "a"
go {[scat v] [ir +]}
went {[scat v] [tense past]} "go&PAST"

When adding new entries to the lexicon it is usually sufficient to enter the citation form
of the word, along with the syntactic category information, as in the illustration for the
word “a” in the preceding examples. When working with languages other than English,
you may wish to add English glosses and even special character sets to the lexicon. For
example, in Cantonese, you could have this entry:
ping4gwo2 {[scat n]} =apple=

To illustrate this, here is an example of the MOR output for an utterance from Cantonese:
*CHI: sik6 ping4gwo2 caang2 hoeng1ziu1 .
%mor: v|sik6=eat n|ping4gwo2=apple
n|caang2=orange n|hoeng1ziu1=banana .

In languages that use both Roman and non-Roman scripts, such as Chinese, you may also
want to add non-Roman characters after the English gloss. This can be done using this
form in which the $ sign separates the English gloss from the representation in characters.
pinyin {[scat x]} "lemmatization" =gloss$characters=

MOR will take the forms indicated by the lemmatization, the gloss, and the characters
and append them after the category representation in the output. The gloss should not
contain spaces or the morpheme delimiters +, -, and #. Instead of spaces or the + sign,
you can use the underscore character to represent compounds.

5.1 Lexicon Building


When running the mor +xb command, you may wish to run the command in the form
mor +xl. The +xb form lists each separate token of an unrecognized word, whereas the
+xl form combines all the tokens into a single type. The advantage of the +xb format is
that you can click on each occurrence and change it. However, for very common errors,
the +xl format is useful because it will allow you to see what forms should be changed
globally using the CHSTRING command.

When working with the output of ther +xb form, you must then go through this file
and determine whether to discard, complete, or modify each missing case. For example, it
may be impossible to decide what category “ta” belongs to without examining where it
Part 3: Morphosyntax 30

occurs in the corpus. In this example, a scan of the Sarah files in the Brown corpus (from
which these examples were taken), reveals that “ta” is a variant of the infinitive marker
“to”:

*MEL: yeah # (be)cause if it's gon (t)a be a p@l it's


got ta go that way.

This missing form can be repaired by joining got and ta into gotta, because that form is
listed in the lexicon. Alternatively, the sequence can be coded as here:
*MEL: yeah # (be)cause if it's gon (t)a be a p@l it's
gotta [: got to] go that way.

Another common source of error is misspelling, which can be corrected.


In many other cases, you will find that some words are just missing from the lexicon.
For these, you can create a file with a name like 0morewords.cut which you add to the
files in /lex. After doing this, please send the contents of this file to macw@cmu.edu, so
that I can add these missing words to the authoritative version of the lexicon.

5.2 Disambiguator Mode


When POST works smoothly, there is littlel need for hand disambiguation.
However, ambiguities within a given part of speech cannot be resolved by POST and
must be disambiguated by hand using Disambiguator Mode. Also, when developing
POST for a new language, you may find this tool useful. Toggling the Disambiguator
Mode option in the Mode menu (escape-2) allows you to go back and forth between
Disambiguator Mode and standard Editor Mode. In Disambiguator Mode, you will see
each ambiguous interpretation on a %mor line broken into its alternative possibilities at
the bottom of the editor screen. The user double-clicks on the correct option and it is in-
serted. An ambiguous entry is defined as any entry that has the ^ symbol in it. For
example, the form N|back^Prep|back is ambiguously either the noun “back” or the
preposition “back.”
When opening a new file for disambiguation using the escape-2 function, make sure
that your cursor is at the beginning of the file. If your cursor is placed after the last
ambiguity, the function will say that you are "Done" even though ambiguities still
remain.
By default, Disambiguator Mode is set to work on the %mor tier. However, you may
find it useful for other tiers as well. To change its tier setting, select the Edit menu and
pull down to Options to get the Options dialog box. Set the disambiguation tier to the
tier you want to disambiguate. To test all of this out, edit the sample.cha file, reset your
default tier, and then type Esc-2. The editor should take you to the second %spa line
which has:
%spa: $RES:sel:ve^$DES:tes:ve

At the bottom of the screen, you will have a choice of two options to select. Once the
correct one is highlighted, you hit a carriage return and the correct alternative will be
inserted. If you find it impossible to decide between alternative tags, you can select the
UND or undecided tag, which will produce a form such as “und|drink” for the word
drink, when you are not sure whether it is a noun or a verb.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 31

6 A Formal Description of the Rule Files


Users working with languages for which grammar files have already been built do
not need to concern themselves with this section or the next. However, users who need to
develop grammars for new languages or who find they need to modify grammars for
existing ones will need to understand how to create the two basic rule files themselves.
You do not need to create a new version of the sf.cut file for special form markers. You
just copy this file from the English MOR grammar.
To build new versions of the arules and crules files for your language, you will need
to study the English files or files for a related language. For example, when you are
building a grammar for Portuguese, it would be helpful to study the grammar that has al-
ready been constructed for Spanish. This section will help you understand the basic prin-
ciples underlying the construction of the arules and crules.

6.1 Declarative structure


Both arules and crules are written using a simple declarative notation. The following
formatting conventions are used throughout:
1. Statements are one per line. Statements can be broken across lines by placing the
continuation character \ at the end of the line.
2. Comments begin with a % character and are terminated by the new line. Comments
may be placed after a statement on the same line, or they may be placed on a
separate line.
3. Names are composed of alphanumeric symbols, plus these characters:
^&+-_:\@./
Both arule and crule files contain a series of rules. Rules contain one or more clauses,
each of which is composed of a series of condition statements, followed by a series of
action statements. For a clause to apply, the input(s) must satisfy all condition state-
ments. The output is derived from the input via the sequential application of all the action
statements.
Both condition and action statements take the form of equations. The left hand side
of the equation is a keyword, which identifies the part of the input or output being
processed. The right-hand side of the rule describes either the surface patterns to be
matched or generated, or the category information that must be checked or manipulated.
The analyzer manipulates two different kinds of information: information about the
surface shape of a word, and information about its category. All statements that match or
manipulate category information must make explicit reference to a feature or features.
Similarly, it is possible for a rule to contain a literal specification of the shape of a stem
or affix. In addition, it is possible to use a pattern matching language to give a more
general description of the shape of a string.

6.2 Pattern-matching symbols


The specification of orthographic patterns relies on a set of symbols derived from the
regular expression (regexp) system in Unix. The rules of this system are:
1. The metacharacters are: * [ ] | . ! All other characters are interpreted literally.
2. A pattern that contains no metacharacters will only match itself, for example the
Part 3: Morphosyntax 32

pattern “abc” will match only the string “abc”.


3. The period matches any character.
4. The asterisk * allows any number of matches (including 0) on the preceding
character. For example, the pattern '.*' will match a string consisting of any number
of characters.
5. The brackets [ ] are used to indicate choice from among a set of characters. The
pattern [ab] will match either a or b.
6. A pattern may consist of a disjunctive choice between two patterns, by use of the |
symbol. For example, the pattern will match all strings which end in x, s, sh, or ch.
7. It is possible to check that some input does not match a pattern by prefacing the
entire pattern with the negation operator !.

6.3 Variable notation


A variable is used to name a regular expression and to record patterns that match it.
A variable must first be declared in a special variable declaration statement. Variable
declaration statements have the format: “VARNAME = regular-expression” where
VARNAME is at most eight characters long. If the variable name is more than one
character, this name should be enclosed in parenthesis when the variable is invoked.
Variables are particularly important for the arules in the ar.cut file. In these rules, the
negation operator is the up arrow ^, not the exclamation mark. Variables may be
declared through combinations of two types of disjunction markers, as in this example for
the definition of a consonant cluster in the English ar.cut file:
O = [^aeiou]|[^aeiou][^aeiou]|[^aeiou][^aeiou][^aeiou]|qu|sq

Here, the square brackets contain the definition of a consonant as not a vowel and the bar
or turnstile symbols separate alternative sequences of one, two, or three consonants.
Then, for good measure, the patterns “qu” and “squ” are also listed as consonantal onsets.
For languages that use combining diacritics and other complex symbols, it is best to use
the turnstile notation, since the square bracket notation assumes single characters. In
these strings, it is important not to include any spaces or tabs, since the presence of a
space will signal the end of the variable.
Once declared, the variable can be invoked in a rule by using the operator $. If the
variable name is longer than a single character, the variable name should be enclosed in
parentheses when invoked. For example, the statement X = .* declares and initializes a
variable named “X.” The name X is entered in a special variable table, along with the
regular expression it stands for. Note that variables may not contain other variables.
The variable table also keeps track of the most recent string that matched a named
pattern. For example, if the variable X is declared as above, then the pattern $Xle will
match all strings that end in le. For example, the string able will match this pattern,
because ab will match the pattern named by X and le will match the literal string le.
Because the string ab is matched against the named pattern X, it will be stored in the
variable table as the most recent instantiation of X, until another string matches X.

6.4 Category Information Operators


The following operators are used to manipulate category information: ADD [feature
Part 3: Morphosyntax 33

value], and DEL [feature value]. These are used in the category action statements. For ex-
ample, the crule statement “RESULTCAT = ADD [num pl]” adds the feature value pair
[num pl] to the result of the concatenation of two morphemes.

6.5 Arules
The function of the arules in the arules.cut file and the additional files in the /ar
folder is to expand the entries in the disk lexicon into a larger number of entries in the on-
line lexicon. Words that undergo regular phonological or orthographic changes when
combined with an affix only need to have one disk lexicon entry. The arules are used to
create on-line lexicon entries for all inflectional variants. These variants are called allos.
For example, the final consonant of the verb “stop” is doubled before a vowel-initial
suffix, such as “-ing.” The disk lexicon contains an entry for “stop,” whereas the online
lexicon contains two entries: one for the form “stop” and one for the form “stopp”.
An arule consists of a header statement, which contains the rulename, followed by
one or more condition-action clauses. Each clause has a series of zero or more conditions
on the input, and one or more sets of actions. Here is an example of a typical condition-
action clause from the larger n-allo rule in the English ar.cut file:
LEX-ENTRY:
LEXSURF = $Yy
LEXCAT = [scat n]
ALLO:
ALLOSURF = $Yie
ALLOCAT = LEXCAT, ADD [allo nYb]
ALLO:
ALLOSURF = LEXSURF
ALLOCAT = LEXCAT, ADD [allo nYa]

This is a single condition-action clause, labeled by the header statement “LEX-ENTRY:”


Conditions begin with one of these two keywords:

1. LEXSURF matches the surface form of the word in the lexical entry to an abstract
pattern. In this case, the variable declaration is
Y = .*[^aeiou]
Given this variable statement, the statement “LEXSURF = $Yy” will match all
lexical entry surfaces that have a final y preceded by a nonvowel.
2. LEXCAT checks the category information given in the matched lexical item against
a given series of feature value pairs, each enclosed in square brackets and separated
by commas. In this case, the rule is meant to apply only to nouns, so the category
information must be [scat n]. It is possible to check that a feature-value pair is not
present by prefacing the feature-value pair with the negation operator !.

Variable declarations should be made at the beginning of the rule, before any of the
condition-action clauses. Variables apply to all following condition-action clauses inside
a rule, but should be redefined for each rule.
After the condition statements come one or more action statements with the label
ALLO: In most cases, one of the action statements is used to create an allomorph and the
other is used to enter the original lexical entry into the run-time lexicon. Action clauses
begin with one of these three keywords:
Part 3: Morphosyntax 34

1. ALLOSURF is used to produce an output surface. An output is a form that will be a


part of the run-time lexicon used in the analysis. In the first action clause, a lexical
entry surface form like “pony” is converted to “ponie” to serve as the stem of the
plural. In the second action clause, the original form “pony” is kept because the
form “ALLOSURF = LEXSURF” causes the surface form of the lexical entry to be
copied over to the surface form of the allo.
2. ALLOCAT determines the category of the output allos. The statement “ALLOCAT
= LEXCAT” causes all category information from the lexical entry to be copied
over to the allo entry. In addition, these two actions add the morphological classes
such as [allo nYa] or [allo nYb] in order to keep track of the nature of these
allomorphs during the application of the crules.
3. ALLOSTEM is used to produce an output stem. This action is not necessary in this
example, because this rule is fully regular and produces a noninflected stem.
However, the arule that converts “postman” into “postmen” uses this ALLOSTEM
action:
ALLOSTEM = $Xman&PL
The result of this action is the form postman&PL that is placed into the %mor line
without the involvement of any of the concatenation rules.

There are two special category feature types that operate to dump the contents of the
arules and the lexicon into the output. These are “gen” and “proc”. The gen feature
introduces its value as a component of the stem. Thus, the entry [gen m] for the Spanish
word “hombre” will end up producing n|hombre&m. The entry [proc dim] for Chinese
reduplicative verbs wil end up producing v|kan4-DIM for the reduplicated form
kan4kan4. These methods allow allorules to directly influence the output of MOR.
Every set of action statements leads to the generation of an additional allomorph for
the online lexicon. Thus, if an arule clause contains several sets of action statements, each
labeled by the header ALLO:, then that arule, when applied to one entry from the disk
lexicon, will result in several entries in the online lexicon. To create the online lexicon,
the arules are applied to the entries in the disk lexicon. Each entry is matched against the
arules in the order in which they occur in the arules file. This ordering of arules is an
extremely important feature. It means that you need to order specific cases before
general cases to avoid having the general case preempt the specific case.
As soon as the input matches all conditions in the condition section of a clause, the
actions are applied to that input to generate one or more allos, which are loaded into the
on-line lexicon. No further rules are applied to that input, and the next entry from the disk
lexicon is then read in to be processed. The complete set of arules should always end with
a default rule to copy over all remaining lexical entries that have not yet been matched by
some rule. This default rule must have this shape:
% default rule- copy input to output
RULENAME: default
LEX-ENTRY:
ALLO:
Part 3: Morphosyntax 35

6.6 Crules
The purpose of the crules in the crules.cut file is to allow stems to combine with
affixes. In these rules, sets of conditions and actions are grouped together into if then
clauses. This allows a rule to apply to a disjunctive set of inputs. As soon as all the
conditions in a clause are met, the actions are carried out. If these are carried out
successfully the rule is considered to have “fired,” and no further clauses in that rule will
be tried.
There are two inputs to a crule: the part of the word identified thus far, called the
“start,” and the next morpheme identified, called the “next.” The best way to think of this
is in terms of a bouncing ball that moves through the word, moving items from the not-
yet-processed chunk on the right over to the already processed chunk on the left. The
output of a crule is called the “result.” The following is the list of the keywords used in
the crules:

keyword function
STARTSURF check surface of start input against some pattern
STARTCAT check start category information
NEXTSURF check surface of next input against some pattern
NEXTCAT check next category information
MATCHCAT check that start and next match for a feature-
value pair type
RESULTCAT output category information

Here is an example of a piece of a rule that uses most of these keywords:


S = .*[sc]h|.*[zxs] % strings that end in affricates
O = .*[^aeiou]o % things that end in o
% clause 1 - special case for "es" suffix
if
STARTSURF = $S
NEXTSURF = es|-es
NEXTCAT = [scat vsfx]
MATCHCAT [allo]
then
RESULTCAT = STARTCAT, NEXTCAT [tense], DEL [allo]
RULEPACKAGE = ()

This rule is used to analyze verbs that end in -es. There are four conditions that must be
matched in this rule:
1. The STARTSURF is a stem that is specified in the declaration to end in an affricate.
The STARTCAT is not defined.
2. The NEXTSURF is the -es suffix that is attached to that stem.
3. The NEXTCAT is the category of the suffix, which is “vsfx” or verbal suffix.
4. The MATCHCAT [allo] statement checks that both the start and next inputs have
the same value for the feature allo. If there are multiple [allo] entries, all must
match.
The shape of the result surface is simply the concatenation of the start and next
surfaces. Hence, it is not necessary to specify this via the crules. The category
information of the result is specified via the RESULTCAT statement. The statement
“RESULTCAT = STARTCAT” causes all category information from the start input to be
Part 3: Morphosyntax 36

copied over to the result. The statement “NEXTCAT [tense]” copies the tense value from
the NEXT to the RESULT and the statement “DEL [allo]” deletes all the values for the
category [allo].
In addition to the condition-action statements, crules include two other statements:
the CTYPE statement, and the RULEPACKAGES statement. The CTYPE statement
identifies the kind of concatenation expected and the way in which this concatenation is
to be marked. This statement follows the RULENAME header. There are two special
CTYPE makers: START and END. “CTYPE: START” is used for those rules that
execute as soon as one morpheme has been found. “CTYPE: END” is used for those rules
that execute when the end of the input has been reached. Otherwise, the CYTPE marker
is used to indicate which concatenation symbol is used when concatenating the
morphemes together into a parse for a word. The # is used between a prefix and a stem, -
is used between a stem and suffix, and ~ is used between a clitic and a stem. In most
cases, rules that specify possible suffixes will start with CTYPE: -. These rules insert a
suffix after the stem.
Rules with CTYPE START are applied as soon as a morpheme has been recognized.
In this case, the beginning of the word is considered as the start input, and the next input
is the morpheme first recognized. As the start input has no surface and no category
information associated with it, conditions and actions are stated only on the next input.
Rules with CTYPE END are invoked when the end of a word is reached, and they are
used to rule out spurious parses. For the endrules, the start input is the entire word that
has just been parsed, and there is no next input. Thus, conditions and actions are only
stated on the start input.
The RULEPACKAGES statement identifies which rules may be applied to the result
of a rule, when that result is the input to another rule. The RULEPACKAGES statement
follows the action statements in a clause. There is a RULEPACKAGES statement associ-
ated with each clause. The rules named in a RULEPACKAGES statement are not tried
until after another morpheme has been found. For example, in parsing the input
“walking”, the parser first finds the morpheme “walk,” and at that point applies the
startrules. Of these startrules, the rule for verbs will be fired. This rule includes a
RULEPACKAGES statement specifying that the rule which handles verb conjugation
may later be fired. When the parser has further identified the morpheme “ing,” the verb
conjugation rule will apply, where “walk” is the start input, and “ing” is the next input.
Note that, unlike the arules which are strictly ordered from top to bottom of the file,
the crules have an order of application that is determined by their CTYPE and the way in
which the RULEPACKAGES statement channels words from one rule to the next.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 37

7 Building new MOR grammars


7.1 minMOR
The simplest possible form of a MOR grammar is represented in the “min” grammar
that you can download from the MOR grammars page at https://childes.talkbank.org.
You can begin your work using with the sample minimal MOR grammars available
from the net. This grammar includes
1. the sf.cut file that all of the MOR grammars use,
2. a sample.cha file with a few words
3. a basically blank ar.cut file, because no allomorphy is yet involved,
4. a cr.cut file that recognizes the parts of speech you will create, along with
one rule for making plural nouns, and
5. a lex folder with examples of verbs, nouns, a determiner, and a suffix.

You can adjust this format to your language and use a different sample.cha file to test out
the operation of this minimal MOR grammar. This system should allow you to build up a
lexicon of uninflected stems. Try to build up separate files for each of the parts of speech
in your language.
You can test out your grammar either by running mor +d sample.cha or else using
interactive MOR with mor +xi. For example, if you use interactive MOR and type
“dogs” you should get
Result: n|dog-PL

7.2 Adding affixes


At some point, you will realize that it would be more efficient to create a system for
lexical analysis, rather than relying only on full forms. This will require you to build up a
morphological grammar. When building a morphology for a new language, it is best to
begin with a paper-and-pencil analysis of the system in which you lay out the various
affixes of the language, the classes of stem allomorphy variations, and the forces that
condition the choices between allomorphs. This work should be guided by a good
descriptive grammar of the morphology of the language. For example, we have used the
Berlitz conjugation books for French, German, Italian, and Spanish.
Once this basic groundwork is finished, you may want to focus on one part-of-speech
at a time. For example, you could begin with the adverbs, since they are often
monomorphemic. Then you could move on to the nouns. The verbs should probably
come last. You can copy the sf.cut file from English and rename it.
As you start to feel comfortable with this, you should begin to add affixes. To do
this, you need to create a lexicon file, such as aff.cut. Using the technique found in
unification grammars, you want to set up categories and allos for these affixes that will
allow them to match up with the right stems when the crules fire. For example, you
might want to call the plural a [scat nsfx] in order to emphasize the fact that it should
attach to nouns. And you could give the designation [allo mdim] to the masculine
diminutive suffix -ito in Spanish in order to make sure that it only attaches to masculine
stems and produces a masculine output.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 38

7.3 Interactive MOR


Once you have a simple lexicon and a set of rule files, you will begin a long process
of working with interactive MOR. When using MOR in the +xi or interactive mode,
there are several additional options that become available in the CLAN Output window.
They are:
word - analyze this word
:q quit- exit program
:c print out current set of crules
:d display application of a rules.
:l re-load rules and lexicon files
:h help - print this message

If you type in a word, such as “dog” or “perro,” MOR will try to analyze it and give you
its component morphemes. If all is well, you can move on the next word. If it is not, you
need to change your rules or the lexicon. You can stay within CLAN and just open these
using the Editor. After you save your changes, use :l to reload and retest.

7.4 Testing
As you begin to elaborate your grammar, you will want to start to work with sets of
files. These can be real data files or else files full of test words. These files provide what
computer scientists call “regression testing”. When you shift to working with files, you
will be combining the use of interactive MOR and the +xi switch with use of the lexicon
testing facility that uses +xl and +xb. As you move through this work, make copies of
your MOR grammar files and lexicon frequently, because you will sometimes find that
you have made a change that makes everything break and you will need to go back to an
earlier stage to figure out what you need to fix. We also recommend using a fast machine
with lots of memory. You will find that you are frequently reloading the grammar using
the :l function, and having a fast machine will speed this process.
As you progress with your work, continually check each new rule change by entering
:l (colon followed by “l” for load) into the CLAN Output window. If you have changed
something in a way that produces a syntactic violation, you will learn this immediately
and be able to change it back. If you find that a method fails, you may need to rethink
your logic. Consider these factors:
1. Arules are strictly ordered. Maybe you have placed a general case before a specific
case.
2. Crules depend on direction from the RULEPACKAGES statement.
3. There must be a START and END rule for each part of speech. If you are getting
too many entries for a word, maybe you have started it twice. Alternatively, you
may have created too many allomorphs with the arules.
4. If you have a MATCHCAT allos statement, all allos must match. The operation
DEL [allo] deletes all allos and you must add back any you want to keep.
5. Make sure that you understand the use of variable notation and pattern matching
symbols for specifying the surface form in the arules.
However, sometimes it is not clear why a method is not working. In this case, you
will want to check the application of the crules using the :c option in the CLAN Output
window. You then need to trace through the firing of the rules. The most important
Part 3: Morphosyntax 39

information is often at the end of this output.


If the stem itself is not being recognized, you will need to also trace the operation of
the arules. To do this, you should either use the +e option in standard MOR or else the :d
option in interactive MOR. The latter is probably the most useful. To use this option,
you should create a directory called testlex with a single file with the words you are
working with. Then run:
mor +xi +ltestlex

Once this runs, type :d and then :l and the output of the arules for this test lexicon will go
to debug.cdc. Use your editor to open that file and try to trace what is happening there.
As you progress with the construction of rules and the enlargement of the lexicon,
you can tackle whole corpora. At this point you will occasionally run the +xl analysis.
Then you take the problems noted by +xl and use them as the basis for repeated testing
using the +xi switch and repeated reloading of the rules as you improve them. As you
build up your rule sets, you will want to annotate them fully using comments preceded by
the % symbol.

7.5 Building Arules


In English, the main arule patterns involve consonant doubling, silent –e, changes of y to
i, and irregulars like “knives” or “leaves.” The rules use the spelling of final consonants
and vowels to predict these various allomorphic variations. Variables such as $V or $C
are set up at the beginning of the file to refer to vowels and consonants and then the rules
use these variables to describe alternative lexical patterns and the shapes of allomorphs.
For example, the rule for consonant doubling takes this shape:
LEX-ENTRY:
LEXSURF = $O$V$C
LEXCAT = [scat v], ![tense OR past perf], ![gem no] % to block
putting
ALLO:
ALLOSURF = $O$V$C$C
ALLOCAT = LEXCAT, ADD [allo vHb]
ALLO:
ALLOSURF = LEXSURF
ALLOCAT = LEXCAT, ADD [allo vHa]

Here, the string $O$V$C characterizes verbs like “bat” that end with vowels
followed by consonants. The first allo will produce words like “batting” or “batter” and
the second will give a stem for “bats” or “bat”. A complete list of allomorphy types for
English is given in the file engcats.cdc in the /docs folder in the MOR grammar.
When a user types the “mor” command to CLAN, the program loads up all the *.cut
files in the lexicon and then passes each lexical form past the rules of the ar.cut file. The
rules in the ar.cut file are strictly ordered. If a form matches a rule, that rule fires and the
allomorphs it produces are encoded into a lexical tree based on a “trie” structure. Then
MOR moves on to the next lexical form, without considering any additional rules. This
means that it is important to place more specific cases before more general cases in a
standard bleeding relation. There is no “feeding” relation in the ar.cut file, since each
form is shipped over to the tree structure after matching.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 40

7.6 Building crules


The other “core” file in a MOR grammar is the cr.cut file that contains the rules that
specify pathways through possible words. The basic idea of crules or concatenation or
continuation rules is taken from Hausser’s (1999) left-associative grammar which
specifies the shape of possible “continuations” as a parser moves from left to right
through a word. Unlike the rules of the ar.cut file, the rules in the cr.cut file are not
ordered. Instead, they work through a “feeding” relation. MOR goes through a candidate
word from left to right to match up the current sequence with forms in the lexical trie
structure. When a match is made, the categories of the current form become a part of the
STARTCAT. If the STARTCAT matches up with the STARTCAT of one of the rules in
cr.cut, as well as satisfying some additional matching conditions specified in the rule,
then that rule fires. The result of this firing is to change the shape of the STARTCAT
and to then thread processing into some additional rules.
For example, let us consider the processing of the verb “reconsidering.” Here, the
first rule to fire is the specific-vpfx-start rule which matches the fact that “re-” has the
feature [scat pfx] and [pcat v]. This initial recognition of the prefix then threads into the
specific-vpfx-verb rule that requires the next item have the feature [scat v]. This rule has
the feature CTYPE # which serves to introduce the # sign into the final tagging to
produce re#part|consider-PRESP. After the verb “consider” is accepted, the
RULEPACKAGE tells MOR to move on to three other rules: v-conj, n:v-deriv, and
adj:v-deriv. Each of these rules can be viewed as a separate thread out of the specific-
vpfx-verb rule. At this point in processing the word, the remaining orthographic material
is “-ing”. Looking at the 0affix.cut file, we see that “ing” has three entries: [scat part],
[scat v:n], and [scat n:gerund]. One of the pathways at this point leads through the v-conj
rule. Within v-conj, only the fourth clause fires, since that clause matches [scat part].
This clause can lead on to three further threads, but, since there is no further orthographic
material, there is no NEXTCAT for these rules. Therefore, this thread then goes on to the
end rules and outputs the first successful parse of “reconsidering.” The second thread
from the specific-vpfx-verb rule leads to the n:v-deriv rule. This rule accepts the reading
of “ing” as [scat n:gerund] to produce the second reading of “reconsidering”. Finally,
MOR traces the third thread from the specific-vpfx-verb rule which leads to adj:v-deriv.
This route produces no matches, so processing terminates with this result:
Result: re#part|consider-PRESP^re#n:gerund|consider-GERUND

Later, POST will work to choose between these two possible readings of
“reconsidering” on the basis of the syntactic context. As we noted earlier, when
“reconsidering” follows an auxiliary (“is eating”) or when it functions adjectivally (“an
eating binge”), it is treated as a participle. However, when it appears as the head of an
NP (“eating is good for you”), it is treated as a gerund. Categories and processes of this
type can be modified to match up with the requirements of the GRASP program to be
discussed below.
The process of building ar.cut and cr.cut files for a new language involves a slow
iteration of lexicon building with rule building. The problem with building up a MOR
grammar one word at a time like this is that changes that favour the analysis of one word
can break the analysis of other words. To make sure that this is not happening, it is
important to have a collection of test words that you continually monitor using mor +xl.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 41

One approach to this is just to have a growing set of transcripts or utterances that can be
analyzed. Another approach is to have a systematic target set configured not as sentences
but as transcripts with one word in each sentence. An example of this approach can be
found in the /verbi folder in the Italian MOR grammar. This folder has one file for each
of the 106 verbal paradigms of the Berlitz Italian Verb Handbook (2005). That handbook
gives the full paradigm of one “leading” verb for each conjugational type. We then typed
all the relevant forms into CHAT files. Then, as we built up the ar.cut file for Italian, we
designed allo types using features that matched the numbers in the Handbook. In the end,
things become a bit more complex in Spanish, Italian, and French.
1. The initial rules of the ar.cut file for these languages specify the most limited and
lexically-bound patterns by listing almost the full stem, as in $Xdice for verbs like
“dicere”, “predicere” or “benedicere” which all behave similarly, or “nuoce” which
is the only verb of its type.
2. Further in the rule list, verbs are listed through a general phonology, but often
limited to the presence of a lexical tag such as [type 16] that indicates verb
membership in a conjugational class.
3. Within the rule for each verb type, the grammar specifies up to 12 stem allomorph
types. Some of these have the same surface phonology. However, to match up
properly across the paradigm, it is important to generate this full set. Once this
basic grid is determined, it is easy to add new rules for each additional conjugational
type by a process of cut-and-paste followed by local modifications.
4. Where possible, the rules are left in an order that corresponds to the order of the
conjugational numbers of the Berlitz Handbook. However, when this order
interferes with rule bleeding, it is changed.
5. Perhaps the biggest conceptual challenge is the formulation of a good set of [allo x]
tags for the paradigm. The current Italian grammar mixes together tags like [allo
vv] that are defined on phonological grounds and tags like [allo vpart] that are
defined on paradigmatic grounds. A more systematic analysis would probably use a
somewhat larger set of tags to cover all tense-aspect-mood slots and use the
phonological tags as a secondary overlay on the basic semantic tags.
6. Although verbs are the major challenge in Romance languages, it is also important
to manage verbal clitics and noun and adjectives plurals. In the end, all nouns must
be listed with gender information. Nouns that have both masculine and feminine
forms are listed with the feature [anim yes] that allows the ar.cut file to generate
both sets of allomorphs.
7. Spanish has additional complexities involving the placement of stress marks for
infinitives and imperatives with suffixed clitics, such as dámelo. Italian has
additional complications for forms such as “nello” and the various pronominal and
clitic forms.

As you progress with your work, continually check each new rule change by entering :l
(colon followed by “l” for load) into the CLAN Output window and then testing some
crucial words. If you have changed something in a way that produces a syntactic
violation, you will learn this immediately and be able to change it back. If you find that a
method fails, you should first rethink your logic. Consider these factors:
1. Arules are strictly ordered. Maybe you have placed a general case before a specific
Part 3: Morphosyntax 42

case.
2. Crules depend on direction from the RULEPACKAGES statement. Perhaps you are
not reaching the rule that needs to fire.
3. There has to be a START and END rule for each part of speech. If you are getting
too many entries for a word, maybe you have started it twice. Alternatively, you
may have created too many allomorphs with the arules.
4. Possibly, you form is not satisfying the requirements of the end rules. If it doesn’t
these rules will not “let it out.”
5. If you have a MATCHCAT allos statement, all allos must match. The operation
DEL [allo] deletes all allos and you must add back any you want to keep.
6. Make sure that you understand the use of variable notation and pattern matching
symbols for specifying the surface form in the arules.
However, sometimes it is not clear why a method is not working. In this case, you
will want to check the application of the crules using the :c option in the CLAN Output
window. You then need to trace through the firing of the rules. The most important
information is often at the end of this output.
If the stem itself is not being recognized, you will need to also trace the operation of
the arules. To do this, you should either use the +e option in standard MOR or else the :d
option in interactive MOR. The latter is probably the most useful. To use this option,
you should create a directory called testlex with a single file with the words you are
working with. Then run: mor +xi +ltestlex
Once this runs, type :d and then :l and the output of the arules for this test lexicon will go
to debug.cdc. Use your editor to open that file and try to trace what is happening there.
As you progress with the construction of rules and the enlargement of the lexicon,
you can tackle whole corpora. At this point you will occasionally run the +xl analysis.
Then you take the problems noted by +xl and use them as the basis for repeated testing
using the +xi switch and repeated reloading of the rules as you improve them. As you
build up your rule sets, you will want to annotate them fully using comments preceded by
the % symbol.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 43

8 MOR for Bilingual Corpora


It is possible to use MOR and POST to process bilingual corpora automatically. A
good sample application of this method is for the transcripts collected by Virginia Yip
and Stephen Matthews from Cantonese-English bilingual children in Hong Kong. In
these corpora, parents, caretakers, and children often switch back and forth between the
two languages. In order to tell MOR which grammar to use for which utterances, each
sentence must be clearly identified for language. By the nature of the goals of the study
and the people conversing with the child, certain files are typically biased toward one
language or the other. In the YipMatthews corpus, English is the default language in
folders such as SophieEng or TimEng and Cantonese is the default in folders such as
SophieCan and TimCan. To mark this in the files in which Cantonese is predominant, the
@Languages tier has this form:
@Language: yue, eng

In the files in which English is predominant, on the other hand, the tier has this form:
@Language: eng, yue

The programs then assume that, by default, each word in the transcript is in the first listed
language. This default can be reversed in two ways. First, within the English files, the
precode [- yue] can be placed at the beginning of utterances that are primarily in
Cantonese. If single Cantonese words are used inside English utterances, they are
marked with the special form marker @s. If an English word appears within a Cantonese
sentence marked with the [- yue] precode, then the @s code means that the default for
that sentence (Chinese) is now reversed to the other language (English). For the files that
are primarily in Cantonese, the opposite pattern is used. In those files, English sentences
are marked as [- eng] and English words inside Cantonese are marked by @s. This form
of marking preserves readability, while still making it clear to the programs which words
are in which language. If it is important to have each word explicitly tagged for
language, the –l switch can be used with CLAN programs such as KWAL, COMBO, or
FIXIT to insert this more verbose method of language marking.
To minimize cross-language listing, it was also helpful to create easy ways of
representing words that were shared between languages. This was particularly important
for the names of family members or relation names. For example, the Cantonese form 姐
姐 for “big sister” can be written in English as Zeze, so that this form can be processed
correctly as a proper noun address term. Similarly, Cantonese has borrowed a set of
English salutations such as “byebye” and “sorry” which are simply added directly to the
Cantonese grammar in the co-eng.cut file.
Once these various adaptations and markings are completed, it is then possible to run
MOR in two passes on the corpus. For the YipMatthews English files, the steps are:
1. To make sure all words in English are recognized, set the MOR library to ENG and
run: mor -s"[- yue]" +xb *.cha
2. Fix any errors and add any missing words to the ENG lexicon.
3. Run: mor -s"[- yue]" *.cha
4. Run CHECK to check for problems.
5. To make sure all words in Cantonese are recognized, set the MOR library to YUE
Part 3: Morphosyntax 44

and run: mor +s"[- yue]" +xb *.cha


6. Fix any errors and add any missing words to the YUE lexicon.
7. Run: mor +s"[- yue]" *.cha
8. Run CHECK to check for problems.

To illustrate the result of this process, here is a representative snippet from the
te951130.cha file in the /TimEng folder. Note that the default language here is English
and that sentences in Cantonese are explicitly marked as [- yue].
*LIN: where is grandma first, tell me ?
%mor: adv:wh|where v|be n|grandma adv|first v|tell pro|me ?
*LIN: well, what's this ?
%mor: co|well pro:wh|what~v|be pro:dem|this ?
*CHI: [- yue] xxx 呢 個 唔 夠 架 .
%mor: unk|xxx det|ni1=this cl|go3=cl neg|m4=not adv|gau3=enough
sfp|gaa3=sfp .
*LIN: [- yue] 呢 個 唔 夠 .
%mor: det|ni1=this cl|go3=cl neg|m4=not adv|gau3=enough .
*LIN: <what does it mean> [>] ?
%mor: pro:wh|what v:aux|do pro|it v|mean ?

This type of analysis is possible whenever MOR grammars exist for both languages, as
would be the case, for example, for Japanese-English, Spanish-English, English-German,
German-French, Spanish-French, Mandarin-Cantonese, or Italian-Mandarin bilinguals.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 45

9 POST
POST was written by Christophe Parisse of INSERM, Paris for automatically
disambiguating the output of MOR. The POST package is composed of four CLAN
commands: POST, POSTTRAIN, POSTLIST, and POSTMOD. POST is the command
that runs the disambiguator. It uses a database called post.db that contains information
about syntactic word order. Databases are created and maintained by POSTTRAIN and
can be dumped in a text file by POSTLIST. POSTMODRULES is a utility for modifying
Brill rules.
There are POST databases now for Cantonese, Danish, English, French, German,
Hebrew, Italian, Japanese, Mandarin, and Spanish. As our work with POST progresses,
we will make these available for additional languages. If you wish to create a post.db
yourself for a new language, please look at the instructions for POSTTRAIN in a later
section of this chapter. To run POST, you can use this command format :
post *.cha

However, POST will also run automatically after running MOR, unless you add the +d
option to MOR. During this process, MOR runs first, then the rules in the prepost.cut file
operate, then POST runs, and finally the rules in postmortem.cut apply.

The accuracy of disambiguation by POST for English, along with prepost.cut and
postmortem.cut, is between 95 and 97 percent. This means that there will be some errors.

The options for POST are:

-b do not use Brill rules (they are used by default)

+bs use a slower but more thorough version of Brill's rules analysis.

+c output all affixes (default)

+cF output the affixes listed in file F and post.db. If there is a posttags.cut file, then
it is used by default as if the +cposttags.cut switch were being used.

-c output only the affixes defined during training with POSTTRAIN

-cF omit the affixes in file F, but not the affixers defined during training with
POSTTRAIN

+dF use POST database file F (default is "post.db"). This file must have been
created by POSTTRAIN. If you do not use this switch, POST will try to locate
a file called post.db in either the current working directory or your MOR
library directory.

+e[1,2]c this option is a complement to the option +s2 and +s3 only. It allows you to
change the separator used (+e1c) between the different solutions, (+e2c) before
Part 3: Morphosyntax 46

the information about the parsing process. (c can be any character). By default,
the separator for +e1 is # and for +e2, the separator is /.

+f send output to file derived from input file name. If you do not use this switch,
POST will create a series of output files named *.pst.

+fF send output to file F. This switch will change the extension to the output files.

-f send output to the screen

+lm reduce memory use (but longer processing time)


+lN when followed by a number the +l switch controls the number of output lines

+unk tries to process unknown words.

+sN N=0 (default) replace ambiguous %mor lines with disambiguated ones
N=1 keep ambiguous %mor lines and add disambiguated %pos lines.
N=2 output as in N=1, but with slashes marking undecidable cases.
N=3 keep ambiguous %mor lines and add %pos lines with debugging info.
N=4 inserts a %nob line before the %mor/%pos line that presents the results of
the analysis without using Brill rules.
N=5 outputs results for debuging POST grammars.
N=6 complete outputs results for debugging POST grammars.
With the options +s0 and +s1, only the best candidate is outputted. With option
+s2, second and following candidates may be outputted, when the
disambiguation process is not able to choose between different solutions with
the most probable solution displayed first. With option +s3, information about
the parsing process is given in three situations: processing of unknown words
(useful for checking these words quickly after the parsing process), processing
of unknown rules and no correct syntactic path obtained (usually corresponds
to new grammatical situations or typographic errors).

+tS include tier code S


-tS exclude tier code S
+/-t#Target_Child - select target child's tiers
+/-t@id="*|Mother|*" - select mother's tiers

9.1 POSTLIST
POSTLIST provides a list of tags used by POST. It is run on the post.db database
file. The options for POSTLIST are as follows:

+dF this gives the name of the database to be listed (default value: ‘eng.db’).
+fF specify name of result file to be F.
+m outputs all the matrix entries present in the database.
+r outputs all the rules present in the database.
+rb outputs rule dictionary for the Brill tagger.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 47

+rn outputs rule dictionary for the Brill tagger in numerical order.
+t outputs the list of all tags present in the database.
+w outputs all the word frequencies gathered in the database.
+wb outputs word dictionary for the Brill tagger.

If none of the options is selected, then general information about the size of the database
is outputted.

Multicat categories provide a way to pack categories together so as to create artificial


categories that have a longer context (four words instead of three) - this makes some
sense from the linguistic point of view, it is a way to consider that clitics are in fact near-
flexions and that the grammatical values is included in the word (and that in fact 'he is' is
to be considered as different from 'he's', which is oral language may not be false).
However if I had to redo all this, I would say the clitic / flexion distinction (whatever the
theoretical interpretation) should in fact be handled at MOR level, not at POST level.
POST should be get the same normalized forms, not hardcode whether they are different
or dissimilar. This would be more language independent.

The +r option outputs rules using the following conventions.


1. pct (punctuation) indicates beginning or end of the sentence.
2. multicat// indicates that the following categories are options
3. the numbers indicate the numbers of contexts for a particular member of the
multicat set, followed by the numbers of occurrences in that context
4. n:gerund|play-GERUND^part|play-PRESP^v:n|play-PRESP => 3 [0,6,0] means that
play has 3 potential categories n:gerund|play-GERUND and part|play-PRESP and
v:n|play-PRESP and that it was found 6 time in the second category in the training
corpus.

9.2 POSTMODRULES
This program outputs the rules used by POST for debugging rules. POSTMODRULES is
used to check and modify Brill rules after initial training.

9.3 PREPOST
PREPOST is not a separate program, but rather a filter that applies during the operation
of MOR. It relies on a file called prepost.cut to automatically prune down the number of
alternative readings created by MOR even before the POST program operates. These
rules assume that the relevant syntactic environment can fully determine the selection
without attention to further information. To illustrate how the rules operate, consider the
case of the English final 's suffix which could be a possessive, a copula, or an auxiliary.
So, the word Ben's have these ambiguities:
n:prop|Ben~aux|be&3S^n:prop|Ben~cop|be&3S^adj|Ben&dn-POSS

However, when this word precedes a noun, it is always a possessive. So, the following
PREPOST rule will select this reading before POST runs:
adj|*-POSS^* n|* => adj|*-POSS n|*
Part 3: Morphosyntax 48

In this rule, the term adj|*-POSS^* indicates any form that has adj|*-POSS as one
reading, because the ^* part says that any other readings are possible. It is also possible
to compose more complex rules that include the *^* terms to indicate any number of
intervening items, as in this example for processing to clean the floor and wash the dishes
v|*^* *^* coord|and v|*^* => v|* *^* coord|and v|*

Here are some additional examples:


# "adj|*" and any number of other ambiguity choices "^*"
det:art|* adj|*^* n|* => det:art|* adj|* n|*

# any number of any "*^*" elements in between


det:dem|*^* *^* n|*^* => det:dem|* n|*

# any number of "adv|*^*" elements in between


aux|*^cop|* (adv|*^*) part|*-PRESP^* => aux|* (adv|*^*) part|*-
PRESP

# next to first element or last element


$b $e

# "$-" means do no change that one element on replacement side at


all
pro:sub|*^* prep|*^adv|* prep|*^adv|* => pro:sub|* $- $-

# skip exactly one element


aux|*^cop|* * part|*-PRESP^* => aux|* $- part|*-PRESP

# skip exactly two elements


aux|*^cop|* * * part|*-PRESP^* => aux|* $- $- part|*-PRESP

# $- with no space inserts a form that wasn’t in the input


prep|*^!conj|*^* adv|here^* => prep|* $-n|here

9.4 POSTMORTEM
This program relies on a dictionary file called postmortem.cut to alter the part-of-speech
tags in the %mor line after the final operation of MOR and POST. The use of this
program is restricted to cases of extreme part-of-speech extension, such as using color
names as nouns or common nouns as verbs. Here is an example of some lines in a
postmortem.cut file

det adj v => det n v


det adj $e => det n $e

Here, the first line will change a sequence such as “the red is” from “det adj v” to “det n
v”. The second line will change “det adj” to “det n” just in the case that the adjective is
at the end of the sentence. The symbol $e represents the end of the utterance.

The rules in POSTMORTEM vary markedly from language to language. They are
Part 3: Morphosyntax 49

particuarly important for German, where they are used to flesh out the morphological
features of the noun phrase.

POSTMORTEM uses the +a switch to run in three possible modes. If no +a switch is


used, then it does all replacements automatically. Second, if you use the +a switch, then
it inserts the new string after the old one and you can then disambiguate the whole file by
hand using escape-2. Third, if you use the +a1 switch, it will work interactively and you
can choose whether to make each replacement on a case by case basis.

9.5 POSTTRAIN
POSTTRAIN was written by Christophe Parisse of INSERM, Paris. In order to run
POST, you need to create a database file for your language. For several languages, this
has already been done. If there is no POST database file for your language or your
subject group, you can use the POSTTRAIN program to create this file. The default
name for this file is post.db. Before running POSTTRAIN, you should take these steps:
1. You should specify a set of files that will be your POSTTRAIN training files. You
may wish to start with a small set of files and then build up as you go.
2. You should verify that all of your training files pass CHECK.
3. Next, you should run MOR with the +xl option to make sure that all words are
recognized.
4. You then run MOR +d on your training files. This will produce an ambiguous
%mor line. Adding the +d switch is necessary to avoid automatic disambiguation.
5. Now you open each file in the editor and use the Esc-2 command to disambiguate
the ambiguous %mor line.
6. Once this is done for a given file, using the Query-Replace function to rename
%mor to %trn.
7. After you have created a few training files or even after you have only one file, run
MOR without the +d switch to create a new disambiguated %mor line to go along
with the %trn line.
8. Now you can run POSTTRAIN with a command like this:
posttrain +c +o0err.cut *.cha
9. Now, take a look at the 0err.cut file to see if there are problems. If not, you can test
out your POST file using POST. If the results seem pretty good, you can shift to
eye-based evaluation of the disambiguated line, rather than using Esc-2. Otherwise,
stick with Esc-2 and create more training data. Whenever you are happy with a
disambiguated %mor line in a new training file, then you can go ahead and rename
it to %trn.
10. The basic idea here is to continue to improve the accuracy of the %trn line as a way
of improving the accuracy of the .db POST database file.

When developing a new POST database, you will find that you need to repeatedly cycle
through a standard set of commands while making continual changes to the input data.
Here is a sample sequence that uses the defaults in POST and POSTTRAIN:

mor *.cha +1
posttrain +c +o0err.cut +x *.cha
Part 3: Morphosyntax 50

post *.cha +1
trnfix *.cha

In these commands, the +1 must be used carefully, since it replaces the original. If a
program crashes or exits while running with +1, the original can be destroyed, so make a
backup of the whole directory first before running +1. TRNFIX can be used to spot
mismatches between the %trn and %mor lines.

The options for POSTTRAIN are:


+a train word frequencies even on utterances longer than length 3.
+b extended learning using Brill's rules
-b Brill's rules training only (However, we never use Brill rules)
+boF append output of Brill rule training to file F (default: send it to screen)
+bN parameter for Brill rules
1- means normal Brill rules are produced (default)
2- means only lexical rules are produced
3- same as +b1, but eliminates rules redundant with binary rules
4- same as +b2, but eliminates rules redundant with binary rules
+btN threshold for Brill rules (default=2). For example, if the value is 2, a rule should
correct 3 errors to be considered useful. To generate all possible rules, use a
threshold of 0.
+c create new POST database file with the name post.db (default)
+cF create new POST database file with the name F
-c add to an existing version of post.db
-cF add to an existing POST database file with the name F
+eF the affixes and stems in file F are used for training. The default name of this file
is tags.cut. So, if you want to add stems for the training, but still keep all
affixes, you will need to add all the affixes explicitly to this list. You must use
the +c switch when using +e.
-e No specific file of affixes and stems isused for training. (This is the default,
unless a tags.cut file is present.)
+mN load the disambiguation matrices into memory (about 700K)
N=0 no matrix training
N=2 training with matrix of size 2
N=3 training with matrix of size 3
N=4 training with matrix of size 4 (default)
+oF append errors output to file F (default: send it to screen)
+sN This switch has three forms
N=0 default log listing mismatches between the %trn and %mor line.
N=1 similar output in a format designed more for developers.
N=2 complete output of all date, including both matches and mismatches
+tS include tier code S
-tS exclude tier code S
+/-t#Target_Child - select target child's tiers
+/-t@id="*|Mother|*" - select mother's tiers
+x use syntactic category suffixes to deal with stem compounds
Part 3: Morphosyntax 51

When using the default switch form of the error log, lines that begin with @ indicate that
the %trn and %mor had different numbers of elements. Lines that do not begin with @
represent simple disagreement between the %trn and the %mor line in some category
assignment. For example, if %mor has pro:dem^pro:exist and %trn has co three times.
Then +s0 would yield: 3 there co (3 {1} pro:dem (2} pro:exist).

By default, POSTTRAIN uses all the affixes in the language and none of the stems. If
you wish to change this behavior, you need to create a file with your grammatical names
for prefixes and suffixes or stem tags. This file can be used by both POSTTRAIN and
POST. However, you may wish to create one file for use by POSTTRAIN and another
for use by POST.

The English POST disambiguator currently achieves over 95% correct disambiguation.
We have not yet computed the levels of accuracy for the other disambiguators. However,
the levels may be a bit better for inflectional languages like Spanish or Italian.

When using TRNFIX, sometimes the %trn will be at fault and sometimes %mor will be
at fault. You can only fix the %trn line. To fix the %mor results, you just have to keep
on compiling more training data by iterating the above process. As a rule of thumb, you
eventually want to have at least 5000 utterances in your training corpus. However, a
corpus with 1000 utterances will be useful initially.

During our work in constructing the training corpus for POSTTRAIN, we bumped into
areas of English grammar where the distinction between parts of speech is difficult to
make without careful specification of detailed criteria. Work with other languages will
probably run into problems of this general type. We can identify four areas that are
particularly problematic in terms of their subsequent effects on GR (grammatical
relation) identification:
1. Adverb vs. preposition vs. particle. The words about, across, “after”, away,
back, down, in, off, on, out, over, and up belong to three categories: ADVerb,
PREPosition and ParTicLe. In practice, it is usually impossible to distinguish a
particle from an adverb. Therefore, we only distinguish adverbs from prepositions.
To distinguish these two, we apply the following criteria. First, a preposition must
have a prepositional object. Second, a preposition forms a constituent with its noun
phrase object, and hence is more closely bound to its object than an adverb or a
particle. Third, prepositional phrases can be fronted, whereas the noun phrases that
happen to follow adverbs or particles cannot. Fourth, a manner adverb can be placed
between the verb and a preposition, but not between a verb and a particle.
2. Verb vs. auxiliary. Distinguishing between Verb and AUXiliary is especially tricky
for the verbs be, do and have. The following tests can be applied. First, if the target
word is accompanied by a nonfinite verb in the same clause, it is an auxiliary, as in I
have had enough or I do not like eggs. Another test that works for these examples is
fronting. In interrogative sentences, the auxiliary is moved to the beginning of the
clause, as in have I had enough? and do I like eggs? whereas main verbs do not
move. In verb-participle constructions headed by the verb be, if the participle is in
Part 3: Morphosyntax 52

the progressive tense (John is smiling), then the head verb is labeled as an
AUXiliary, otherwise it is a Verb (John is happy).
3. Copula vs. auxiliary. A related problem is the distinction between v:cop and aux
for the verb to be. This problem arises mostly when the verb is followed by the past
participle, as in I was finished. For these constructions, we take the approach that
the verb is always the copula, unless there is a by phrase marking the passive.
4. Communicators. COmmunicators can be hard to distinguish imperatives or
locative adverbs, especially at the beginning of a sentence. Consider a sentence such
as there you are where there could be interpreted as either specifying a location vs.
there is a car in which there is pro:exist.

9.6 POSTMOD
This tool enables you to modify the Brill rules of a database. There are these options:
+dF use POST database file F (default is eng.db).
+rF specify name of file (F) containing actions that modify rules.
+c force creation of Brill's rules.
+lm reduce memory use (but increase processing time).

9.7 TRNFIX
By default, this program compares the forms on the %trn line with those on the %mor
line and reports each mismatch. This simple command will run across all the files in a
folder of training files:
trnfix *.cha

You can then triple click on each line for a mismatch to determine whether the error is in
the %mor or the %trn line. If it is in the %trn line, you can fix it and that will improve
your training and the accuracy of POST. TRNFIX can also be used to compare the %gra
and %grt lines using this form:
trnfix +b%gra +b%grt *.cha
Part 3: Morphosyntax 53

10 GRASP – Syntactic Dependency Analysis


This chapter, with contributions from Eric Davis, Shuly Wintner, Brian MacWhinney,
Alon Lavie, Andrew Yankes, and Kenji Sagae, describes a system for coding syntactic
dependencies in the English TalkBank corpora. This chapter explains the annotation
system and describes each of the grammatical relations (GRs) available for tagging
dependency relations.

10.1 Grammatical Relations


GRASP describes the structure of sentences in terms of pairwise grammatical relations
between words. These grammatical relations involve two dimensions: attachment and
valency. In terms of attachment, each pair has a head and a dependent. These
dependency relations are unidirectional and cannot be used to represent bidirectional
relations. Along the valency dimension, each pair has a predicate and an argument. Each
dependency relation is labeled with an arc and the arc has an arrow which points from the
predicate to argument. Valency relations open slots for arguments. In English, modifiers
(adjectives, determiners, quantifiers) are predicates whose arguments are the following
nouns. In this type of dependency organization, the argument becomes the head.
However, in other grammatical relations, the predicate or governor is the head and the
resultant phrase takes on its functions from the predicate. Examples of predicate-head
GRs include the attachment of thematic roles to verbs and the attachment of adjuncts to
their heads.

Here is an example of the coding of the sentence the big dog chased five cats for
dependencies:
*TXT: the big dog chased five cats.
%mor: det|the adj|big n|dog v|chase-PAST quant|five n|cat-PL.
%gra: 1|3|DET 2|3|MOD 3|4|SUBJ 4|0|ROOT 5|6|QUANT 6|4|OBJ

This notation can be described in this way:


1. The determiner the is the first item and it attaches to the third item dog. Here the
determiner is the predicate and the dependent. The GR here is DET or
determination.
2. The adjective big is a predicate that attaches as a dependent of dog. The GR here is
MOD or modification.
3. The noun dog is the head of the phrase the big dog and it attaches as a dependent
subject or SUBJ of the predicate chased. Here we ignore the attachment of the
suffix –ed to the verb.
4. The verb chased is the root of the clause. It attaches to the zero position which is
the “root” of the sentence.
5. The quantifier five attaches to the noun cats through the QUANT relation.
6. The noun cats attaches as a dependent to the verb chased through the OBJ or object
relation.

The following diagram describes the sentence We eat the cheese sandwich graphically
through arcs with arrowheads instead of through the numbering system:
Part 3: Morphosyntax 54

This picture is equivalent to this notation in the numbering system:

*TXT: we eat the cheese sandwich.


%mor: pro|we v|eat det|the n|cheese n|sandwich .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|5|DET 4|5|MOD 5|2|OBJ

The creation of these %gra notations, either by hand or by machine, depends on three
levels of representation.
1. Words must be consistently coded for the correct part of part of speech on the %mor
line. For English, the relevant categories are given in the MOR grammars. The
major categories are adjective, adverb, pronoun, noun, and verb. However, there are
many additional subcategories. Compounds are treated as single units given in their
main part of speech by MOR.
2. GRASP makes use of a large set of grammatical relations (GRs), given below.
3. GRs apply to phrases or clusters of words connected through dependency relations.
The most import phrasal types are: noun phrase, verb phrase, absolute phrase,
gerund phrase, auxiliary phrase, and infinitival phrase. To achieve attachment to
heads, phrases must be represented by one of their component lexical items. In
some structures this can be a relativizer or conjunction; in others, it can be the main
verb of the subordinate clause.
The following is a comprehensive list of the grammatical relations in the GRASP
annotation scheme. Example GRs as well as relations to similar GRs are provided. In this
annotation scheme, C refers to clausal and X refers to non-finite clausal. This list is
divided into relations with the predicate as head and relations with the argument as head.
In the examples, the dependent is marked in italics.

10.2 Predicate-head relations


First, we list the relations in which the dependent attaches to a head that serves as the
predicate. In many of these relations, the head is the verb. The combination of a verb
with its arguments, including the SUBJ argument, constitutes a verb phrase.
1. SUBJect identifies the subject of clause, when the subject itself is not a clause.
Typically, the head is the main verb and the dependent is a nominal. Ex: You eat
with your spoon.
2. ClausalSUBJect = CSUBJ identifies the finite clausal subject of another clause.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 55

The head is the main verb, and the dependent is the main verb of the clausal
subject. An alternative analysis of these structures would treat the subordinator
“that” as the head of the CSUBJ. Ex: That Eric cried moved Bush.
3. OBJect identifies the first object or direct object of a verb. The head is the main
verb, and the dependent is a nominal or a noun that is the head of a nominal
phrase. A clausal complement relation should be denoted by COMP, not OBJ or
OBJ2. Ex: You read the book.
4. OBJect2 = OBJ2 identifies the second object or indirect object of a ditransitive
verb, when not introduced by a preposition. The head is a ditransitive verb, and
the dependent is a noun (or other nominal). The dependent must be the head of a
required non-clausal and nonprepositional complement of a verb (head of OBJ2)
that is also the head of an OBJ relation. Ex: He gave you your telephone. When
the indirect object is in a prepositional phrase, it is just coded as a prepositional
phrase.
5. COMPlement identifies a clausal complement of a verb. The head is the main
verb of the matrix clause, and the dependent is the main verb of the clausal
complement. Examples: I think that was Fraser. You’re going to stand on my toe.
I told you to go. Eve, you stop throwing the blocks.
6. PREDicate identifies a predicate nominal or predicate adjective of verbs such as
be and become. The predicate can also be an embedded clause, and then head of
that clause is the verb. PRED should not be confused with COMP, which
identifies the complement of a verb. If there is a relativizer, it attaches to the verb
of the embedded clause through the LINK relation. Examples: I’m not sure. He is
a doctor. This is how I drink my coffee. My goal is to win the competition.
7. ClausalPrepositionalOBJect = CPOBJ indentifies a full clause that serves as the
object of a preposition. The verb of the clause attaches to the preposition which is
the head. Here, again, the relativizer attaches to the verb of the subordinate clause
through the LINK relation. Ex: I’m confused about what you are asking.
8. ClausalOBJect = COBJ indentifies a full clause that serves as OBJ. The verb of
the object clause attaches to the main verb which is the head. Here, again, the
relativizer attaches to the verb of the subordinate clause through the LINK
relation. Ex: I remember what you said.
9. PrepositionalOBJect = POBJ is the relation between a preposition and its object.
The head is a preposition, and the dependent is typically a noun. The traditional
treatment of the prepositional phrase views the object of the preposition as the
head of the prepositional phrase. However, we are here treating the preposition as
the head, since the prepositional phrase then participates in a further JCT relation
to a head verb or a NJCT relation to a head noun. Ex: You want to sit on the
stool?
10. SeRiaL identifies serial verbs such as go play and come see. In English, such verb
sequences start with either come or go. The initial verb is the dependent, and the
following verb is the head and typically the root of the sentence. Ex: Come see if
we can find it. Go play with your toys over there. This relation can also be used
when children overuse the pattern in forms with omitted infinitives such as want
see, try go. It is not used with auxiliary have, although sometimes it seems that it
could be.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 56

10.3 Argument-head relations


Relations in which the arguments (rather than the predicates) serve as the heads include
relations of adjunction and modification.
1. adJunCT = JCT identifies an adjunct that modifies a verb, adjective, or adverb.
This grammatical relation covers a wide variety of structures whose exact form is
best understood by noting the specific parts of speech that are involved. In these,
the adjunct is the predicate, since it opens a valency slot for something to attach
to. The head of JCT is the verb, adjective or adverb to which the JCT attaches as
a dependent. The dependent is typically an adverb or a preposition (in the case of
phrasal adjuncts headed by a preposition, such as a prepositional phrase).
Occasionally, a locative noun may function as an adjunct, as with way in he was
going all the way home. Adjuncts are optional and carry meaning on their own
(and do not change the basic meaning of their JCT heads). Verbs requiring a
complement describing location may be treated as prepositional objects, in which
case the IOBJ relation applies (see above). Ex: That’s much better. He ran with a
limp. That’s really big.
2. ClausalconJunCT = CJCT identifies a finite clause that adjoins coordinatively to a
verb, adjective, or adverb head. The dependent is the main verb of the subordinate
clause and this clause attaches to the root verb of the main clause. The
conjunction uses the LINK relation and attaches to the verb of the subordinate
clause. Ex: We can’t find it, because it is gone. When two clauses are combined
with and CJCT is the link between the main verb of the second clause and the
first. However, this relation should not be used if the subject is not repeated, as in
John runs 5 miles, and swims one every day. Instead, this is a case of a
coordinated verb.
3. XadJunCT = XJCT identifies a non-finite clause that attaches to a verb, adjective,
or adverb. The dependent is typically the main verb of a non-finite subordinate
clause. There is usually no conjunction for these. Ex: Running to the carriage, she
lost her slipper. Note: this construction can be confused with ones in which a PP
or adverb intervenes between an auxiliary and its participle, as in she’s outside
sleeping in the carriage. However, when the main verb is a copula, as in there’s a
man sleeping in the car, then XJCT is appropriate.
4. Nominal adJunCT = NJCT identifies the head of a complex NP with a
prepositional phrase attached as an adjunct of a noun. Note that, if a prepositional
phrase attaches to a verb, the relation is JCT and not NJCT. In a sense, this
relation is a cross between JCT and MOD. Ex: The man with an umbrella arrived
late.
5. MODifier identifies a non-clausal nominal modifier. This should not be confused
with the part of speech code with the same name for modals. The link of modals
to the main verb is coded as AUX. The head is a noun, and the dependent is
typically an adjective or another noun, including a possessive. Ex: Would you like
grape juice? That’s a nice box. That’s John’s stick.
6. POSTMODifier identifies a postposed nominal modifier. The head is a noun, and
the dependent is a following adjective. Ex: I will dig a hole bigger than a foot.
Often these express resultative relations, as in I painted the barn red. Sometimes
they occur after intervening material, as in He pulled her out of the water safe.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 57

7. POSSessive is used for the relation between the English possessive suffix and the
head noun. The head is the noun. The introduction of this relation is required to
avoid problems in processing the verb clitics that take the same phonological
shape. Ex: John’s book.
8. APPositive identifies the relation between an appositive phrase and its head. Ex:
Barack Obama, President of the United States. It can also be used to link a
topicalized noun to a following pronoun. Ex: John, he really likes waffles.
9. ClausalMODifier = CMOD identifies a finite clause that is a nominal modifier
(such as a relative clause) or complement. The head is a noun, and the dependent
is typically a finite verb. Ex: Here are the grapes I found. This relation can also be
used when the head is an adjective. Ex: He was happy he found the girl.
10. XMODifier identifies a non-finite clause that is a nominal modifier (such as a
relative clause) or complement. The head is a noun, and the dependent is typically
a non-finite verb. Ex: It’s time to take a nap. I saw a man running away.
11. DETerminer identifies the relation between a determiner and its head noun.
Determiners include the, a, as well as (adjectival) possessives pronouns (my, your,
etc) and demonstratives (this, those, etc), but not quantifiers (all, some, any, etc;
see QUANT below). Typically, the head is a noun and the dependent/governor is
a determiner. In cases where a word that is usually a determiner does not have a
head, there is no DET relation. Ex: I want that cookie.
12. QUANTifier identifies a nominal quantifier, such as three, many, and some.
Typically, the head is a noun, and the dependent is a quantifier, or sometimes an
adverb. In cases where a quantifier has no head, there is no QUANT relation. In
English, the MOD, DET, and QUANT relations have largely the same syntax.
However, within the noun phrase, we occasionally see that they are ordered as
DET+QUANT+MOD+N. Ex: These are my three ripe bananas.
13. PostQuantifier = PQ is the relation between a postquantifier and the preceding
head nominal. Ex: We both arrived late.
14. AUXiliary identifies an auxiliary or modal of a main verb. The head is a verb, and
the dependent is an auxiliary (such as be or have) or a modal (such as can or
should). Ex: Can you do it?
15. NEGation identifies verbal negation. When the word not (contracted or not)
follows an auxiliary or modal (or sometimes a verb), it is the dependent in a NEG
relation (not JCT), where the auxiliary, modal or verb (in the absence of an
auxiliary or modal) is the head. Ex: Mommy will not read it.
16. INFinitive identifies the relation between the infinitival particle (to) and the verb
to which it attaches. The head is a verb, and the dependent is always to. Ex: He’s
going to drink the coffee.
17. LINK identifies the relation between a complementizer (that), relativizer (who,
which) or subordinate conjunction (including and) and the verb in the subordinate
clause to which it attaches. The verb of the subordinate clause attaches to the
main verb in a CJCT, CMOD, COBJ, CPOBJ, PRED, or COMP relation. Ex:
Wait until the noodles are cool.
18. TAG is the relation between the finite verb of a tag question and the root verb of
the main clause. Ex: You know how to count, don’t you? English and Portuguese
have this structure, but many other languages do not.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 58

10.4 Extra-clausal elements


In these relations, the dependent is a clausal modifier. These should be linked to the root.
1. COMmunicator identifies a communicator (such as hey, okay, etc) or a vocative
when it occurs inside an utterance. When these occur at the beginning or end of
utterances, then use BEG and END instead. The head of COM is the ROOT.
2. BEGin identifies an initial clause-external element, such as a vocative or topic.
The head of BEG is “0”. Ex: Eve, are you coming? BEG is marked in the main
line with a following ‡ mark that is coded on the %mor line as beg|begp. The
BEGP relation is linked to BEG.
3. END identifies a final clause-external postposed element, including sentence final
particles, final vocatives, final interactionals, and single word tags like right?
Like COM, but unlike BEG, the head of the END is the ROOT. This is done,
because the parser works better in this way. END is marked in the main line with
a preceding „ mark that is coded on the %mor line as end|endp. Ex: Some more
cookies, Eve?
4. INCompleteROOT identifies a word that serves as the root of an utterance,
because the usual root forms (verbs in English) are missing. This form could be a
single word by itself (adverb, communicator, noun, adjective) or a word with
additional modifiers, such as the noun dog in the big dog, when it occurs by itself
without a verb. It may appear that there could be more than one of these in an
utterance, as in well, sure. However, in this case, well should be marked as a CO
that is dependent on sure.
5. OMission is used when ellipsis or omission leaves determiners or other modifiers
without heads. They should then be attached to the most local predicate using the
OM relation.

10.5 Cosmetic relations


There are several relations that are just used during transcription to assist in the accurate
training of the GRASP tagger:
1. PUNCTuation is the relation between the final punctuation mark and ROOT.
2. LP (local punctuation) is the relation between quotes or commas and overall
structure. Commas delimiting clauses should be linked to the root. Each comma
in an ENUM series should be linked to the previous word. Beginning and ending
quote marks should be linked to “0”.
3. BEGP is the relation between the ‡ mark and the BEG.
4. ENDP is the relation between the „ mark and the END.
5. ROOT This is the relation between the topmost word in a sentence (the root of the
dependency tree) and the LeftWall or “0”. The topmost word in a sentence is the
word that is the head of one or more relations, but which is not the dependent in
any relation with other words (except for the LeftWall).

Series relations. Some additional relations involve processes of listing, coordination,


and classification. In some of these, the final element is the head and the initial elements
all depend on the final head. However, in coordinations, we take the first element as the
head.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 59

1. NAME identifies a string of proper names such as Eric Davis and New York
Central Library. The initial name is the dependent, and the following name is the
head. Ex: My name is Tom Jones.
2. DATE identifies a date with month and year, month and day, or month, day, and
year. Examples include October 7, 1980 and July 26. For consistency with
compounds and NAME, we regard the final element in these various forms as the
head. Ex: October seventh nineteen ninety.
3. ENUMeration involves a relation between elements in a series without any
coordination based on a conjunction (and, but, or). The series can contain letters,
numbers, and nominals. The head is the first item in the series, and all the other
items in the enumeration depend on this first word. Ex: one, two, three, four.
4. CONJ involves a relation between a coordinating conjunction and the cluster of
preceding conjoined items that has been combined with ENUM. For example, in
the phrase I walk, jump, and run, the items walk and jump are combined by
ENUM so that walk is the head. The conjunction and then attaches to walk with
the CONJ relation. The resultant phrase walk, jump and is then further linked by
the COORD relation to the final element ran. Ex: I walk, jump, and run.
5. COORD involves an attachment of a final coordinated element to the conjunction.
For example, in the sentence I walk, jump, and run, the verb run is attached to
and. Note: When the word and functions as a subordinating conjunction, it is
treated as having a LINK relation to the verb of its clause. In this case, it is not
conjoining phrases but whole clauses. In the neither X nor Y and either X or Y
constructions, the words neither, nor, either, and or are treated as coord by MOR
and each coord words links to the following content word (noun, verb, or
adjective) with COORD. Then the first content word links to the second with
ENUM.

10.6 MEGRASP
MEGRASP is the CLAN command that creates a %gra line based on the forms in the
%mor line. MEGRASP uses a maximum entropy classifier system encoded in the
megrasp.mod database file to create a dependency parsing based on binary grammatical
relations. To visualize these dependencies graphically, you can triple click on a %gra line
and CLAN will run a web service that produces labelled graph structure on your
computer screen. The figures in the next chapter are screengrabs taken from those
displays.
To create a new megrasp.mod database file, you can run MEGRASP in training
mode used the –t switch. In that case, your training corpus must contain a gold standard
%grt tier to control training. The program uses these options:
-e : evaluate accuracy (input file must contain gold standard GRs)
-t : training mode (parser runs in parse mode by default)
-iN: number of iterations for training ME model (default: 300)
-cN: inequality parameter for training ME model (default: 0.5).
+fS: send output to file (program will derive filename)
+re: run program recursively on all sub-directories.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 60

11 Building a training corpus


Once a megrasp.mod file has been created by running MEGRASP in training mode, the
creation of the %gra line follows automatically in the chain of MOR-PREPOST-POST-
POSTMORTEM-MEGRASP. However, to produce accurate tagging it is important to
have properly tagged GRs in the training corpus. To do this accurately involves making
consistent decisions for certain GRs that are easy to confuse. This chapter provides
illustrations that can help in making these choices between competing GR assignments.
These illustrations were produced using the GraphViz system, which we run as a web
service. If your computer is connected to the web, you can double click on a %gra line in
a transcript and the service will produce one of these graphs which you can use to verify
the accuracy of the grammatical relations.

11.1 ROOT, SUBJ, DET, PUNCT

ROOT identifies the topmost verb of a sentence, which doesn’t depend upon any other
element in the sentence.
The head of ROOT is 0, the LeftWall.

SUBJect identifies the subject of a verb, as long as that subject is a noun phrase. (If the
subject is instead a verbal clause, it’s tagged differently – see section 13.)
The head of SUBJ is the verb for which it is acting as a subject.

DETeterminer identifies the determiner of a noun phrase. This can be a definite or


indefinite article (the, an); a possessive pronoun (my, your); or a demonstrative (this,
those). However, it doesn’t include quantifiers like many and related categories (see
section 12).
The head of DET is the noun to which it attaches.

PUNCTuation identifies the punctuation mark at the end of a sentence (for initial and
medial punctuation, see section 16).
The head of PUNCT is the ROOT.

Example 1a
Part 3: Morphosyntax 61

*PAR: the frog jumped .


%mor: det:art|the n|frog v|jump-PAST .
%gra: 1|2|DET 2|3|SUBJ 3|0|ROOT 4|3|PUNCT

Here there is only one verb, jumped, clearly functioning as the topmost verb of the
sentence. So it’s identified as the ROOT and tied to 0, and the PUNCT ties to jumped in
turn. frog serves as the subject of the verb, so it ties to jumped as a SUBJ. Finally, the ties
as a DET to the noun which it specifies.

11.2 OBJ and OBJ2


OBJect identifies the direct object or first object of a verb. As with SUBJ, OBJ should
be a nominal phrase. If it is a clause, it is COBJ. The head of OBJ is the verb for which it
is acting as a direct object.

*PAR: the worker pushes the box .


%mor: art|the n|work&dv-AGT v|push-3S art|the n|box .
%gra: 1|2|DET 2|3|SUBJ 3|0|ROOT 4|5|DET 5|3|OBJ 6|3|PUNCT

OBJect2 identifies the indirect object or second object of a verb, if it is not introduced by
a preposition. The head of OBJ2, like OBJ, is the verb for which it’s acting as an indirect
object. Here is an example with both OBJ and OBJ2:

*PAR: the child gave the dog a treat .


%mor: art|the n|child v|give-PAST art|the n|dog art|a n|treat .
%gra: 1|2|DET 2|3|SUBJ 3|0|ROOT 4|5|DET 5|3|OBJ2 6|7|DET 7|3|OBJ 8|3|PUNCT

The verb gave takes three arguments here. The SUBJ is child, the OBJ is treat and the
OBJ2 is dog. OBJ2 is only used in case such as this where the beneficiary is not included
in a prepositional phrase, as in the child gave a treat to the dog. In this latter case, to the
dog is treated as an adjunct to the verb. Thus to ties as a JCT to gave, and dog ties as a
POBJ to to.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 62

*PAR: the child gave a treat to the dog .


%mor: art|the n|child v|give-PAST art|a n|treat prep|to det|the n|dog .
%gra: 1|2|DET 2|3|SUBJ 3|0|ROOT 4|5|DET 5|3|OBJ 6|3|JCT 7|8|DET 8|6|POBJ 9|3|PUNCT

11.3 JCT, NJCT and POBJ


adJunCT = JCT identifies an adjunct that modifies a verb, adjective, or adverb (but not
a noun – see section 4). The head of JCT is the verb, adjective, or adverb it modifies. This
covers a very wide range of grammatical structures, but the word which is the dependent
in the JCT relation is typically a preposition or adverb. Differentiating the type of JCT
can be done by using searches that include the part of speech of the head on the %mor
line. JCTs very commonly head up prepositional phrases. (A JCT may also be a noun in
some cases, such as way in she walked all the way home.)

Nominal adJunCT = NJCT identifies an adjunct that modifies a noun rather than a verb,
adjective, or adverb. Otherwise it behaves the same as JCT, able to take a POBJ and so
on. The head of NJCT is the noun it modifies. Here is an example:

*PAR: she is a friend of my sister .


%mor: pro:sub|she cop|be&3S art|a n|friend prep|of pro:poss:det|my n|sister .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|4|DET 4|2|PRED 5|4|NJCT 6|7|DET 7|5|POBJ 8|2|PUNCT

Prepositional OBJect = POBJ identifies the non-clausal nominal object of a


preposition, such as town in the phrase around town. Clausal preposition objects are
coded as CPOBJ. The head of POBJ is the preposition on which the nominal object
depends.

Note: JCT and POBJ also work together to describe phrasal verbs, despite the fact that
their function is not truly adjunctive. For example, in the sentence he took out the trash,
out is tied to took as a JCT and trash is tied to out as a POBJ.
This isn’t a strictly accurate parse of the sentence, since out the trash is not an optional
constituent here; rather, the trash would be more properly understood as an OBJ to the
phrasal verb took out. However, because of the difficulties GRASP has in disambiguating
these two cases, it is more consistent to treat this as a JCT-POBJ construction.

Note: When a prepositional phrase acts as the predicate in a be construction, treat it as a


Part 3: Morphosyntax 63

JCT (and a POBJ, if there is one). A simple example would be she is in the boathouse.
You might find it counterintuitive to tie in to is as a JCT there, since in the boathouse
is, again, not an optional constituent. (As you’ll see in the next section, it could be
tempting to call in the boathouse a PRED.) It’s more consistent to treat prepositional
phrases this way, though, even when they’re not adjunctive.

*PAR: I really messed up that test.


%mor: pro:sub|I adv|real&dadj-LY v|mess-PAST prep|up det|that n|test .
%gra: 1|3|SUBJ 2|3|JCT 3|0|ROOT 4|3|JCT 5|6|DET 6|4|POBJ 7|3|PUNCT

First, note the adverb really tying as a JCT to the verb it modifies, messed. Otherwise, the
purpose of this example is demonstrating how to diagram a phrasal verb in GRASP. As
you can see, the way to handle the phrasal verb messed up is to treat up that test as an
adjunct to messed, even though that’s a slight compromise in accuracy.

*PAR: Peter is outside the tower .


%mor: n:prop|Peter cop|be&3S prep|outside art|the n|tower .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|2|JCT 4|5|DET 5|3|POBJ 6|2|PUNCT

In this subject-be-PP construction. outside the tower is treated as a JCT to is in order to


maximize consistency in the treatment of prepositional phrases, allowing for the fact that
this leaves the copula is in the strange position of not appearing to take any predicate.
Such structures can be searched by looking for copulas linked POBJ.

11.4 PRED
PREDicate identifies a nominal or adjectival predicate. This is usually an argument of
the verb be, although other verbs such as become and grow (in the sense of I grow weary
of your complaints) can also take a predicate. The head of PRED is the verb of which it is
an argument. Here is a simple adjectival predicate.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 64

*PAR: you became wiser .


%mor: pro:sub|you v|become-PAST adj|wise-CP .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|2|PRED 4|2|PUNCT

PRED also participates along with SUBJ in there’s-an-X constructions. Treat there as an
existential pronoun in these constructions, tying it to the be verb as a SUBJ. Then tie the
introduced noun phrase to the be verb as a PRED. (Do the same for here’s-an-X.) Here is
an example:

*PAR: there's a ghost in the cloakroom .


%mor: pro:exist|there~cop|be&3S art|a n|ghost prep|in art|the n|+n|cloak+n|room .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|4|DET 4|2|PRED 5|4|NJCT 6|7|DET 7|5|POBJ 8|2|PUNCT

11.5 AUX
AUXiliary identifies an auxiliary or modal of a main verb, such as can or should. The
head of AUX is the lexical verb. When multiple AUXes are present (could have guessed),
each again ties individually to the main verb, for consistency. (None of the AUXes tie to
each other.) Here is a simple auxiliary construction. Although can carries the tense
information, the ROOT of the sentence is still the main verb, help. As mentioned above,
lawyer ties directly to help as the SUBJ (it does not tie to can, the AUX).

*PAR: the lawyer can help us .


%mor: art|the n|lawyer mod|can v|help pro:obj|us .
%gra: 1|2|DET 2|4|SUBJ 3|4|AUX 4|0|ROOT 5|4|OBJ 6|4|PUNCT
Part 3: Morphosyntax 65

11.6 NEG
NEGation identifies a verbal negator; i.e. not (including its contracted form -n’t). In
modern English, the presence of a NEG is nearly always accompanied by an AUX, a
form of be as a main verb (she isn’t friendly), or sometimes a participle (consider not
telling anyone that). NEG can also in some cases negate a relativizer (see section 10 for
details on this). The head of NEG is the AUX, verb, or relativizer it negates.

*PAR: don't you enjoy the theatre ?


%mor: mod|do~neg|not pro:sub|you v|enjoy art|the n|theatre ?
%gra: 1|4|AUX 2|1|NEG 3|4|SUBJ 4|0|ROOT 5|6|DET 6|4|OBJ 7|4|PUNCT

This demonstrates an AUX-leading question as well as a negated AUX. Structurally it’s


no different from you don’t enjoy the theatre, just rearranged. The NEG ties to the AUX,
not the main verb enjoy.

11.7 MOD and POSS


MODifier identifies a non-clausal modifier which precedes the noun it modifies. This is
usually an adjective, a possessive, or another noun (as in the cookie factory, where cookie
is a MOD to factory). The head of MOD is the noun being modified. When multiple
MODs modify the same noun, each one ties to that noun separately. Here is a simple
MOD construction. Both the DET a and the MOD new tie individually to apartment (a
does not tie to new).

*PAR: then we'll find a new apartment .


%mor: adv:tem|then pro:sub|we~mod|will v|find art|a adj|new n|apartment .
%gra: 1|4|JCT 2|4|SUBJ 3|4|AUX 4|0|ROOT 5|7|DET 6|7|MOD 7|4|OBJ 8|4|PUNCT

POSSessive identifies the relation between the English possessive suffix and the noun it
affixes to, such as -’s in John’s book. The head of POSS is the noun to which it attaches
(John in the given example, not book). The affixed noun then ties as a MOD to the noun
it specifies (so John ties to book). In this next example, -’s ties to Sarah as a POSS,
while Sarah ties to piece as a MOD (greatest also ties to piece as a separate MOD).
Part 3: Morphosyntax 66

*PAR: this is Sarah's greatest piece in the exhibition .


%mor: pro:dem|this cop|be&3S n:prop|Sarah~poss|s adj|great&SP n|piece
prep|in art|the n|exhibition .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|6|MOD 4|3|POSS 5|6|MOD 6|2|PRED 7|6|NJCT 8|9|DET
9|7|POBJ 10|2|ROOT

11.8 CONJ and COORD


The relations of CONJ and COORD only apply to non-clausal coordination or
combination. These relations use the coordinators and and or ([scat coord]). The use of
conjunctions ([scat conj]) to link subordinate clauses with main clauses is described by
the LINK relation, discussed later. CONJ and COORD work together in the following
way.

COORD first links the last element of a conjoined series to the coordinator.

CONJ then links the coordinator to the preceding element or enumerated element. For
example, in the phrase bold and eloquent woman, first eloquent ties to and as a COORD,
then and ties to bold as a CONJ, finally bold ties to woman as a MOD. The overall effect
is to unite bold and eloquent into one constituent headed by its first element, bold which
then modifies woman.

Here is an example of a sentence with two CONJ-COORD constructions. There are three
conjoined verb phrases. The first one, scrimped, serves as the ROOT, while the other
two, saved and bought, are first combined through COORD and then linked to the main
verb through CONJ.

*PAR: she scrimped and saved and bought a car .


%mor: pro:sub|she v|scrimp-PAST conj|and v|save-PAST conj|and v|buy-PAST art|a n|car .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|2|CONJ 4|3|COORD 5|2|CONJ 6|5|COORD 7|8|DET 8|6|OBJ
9|2|PUNCT

11.9 ENUM
ENUMeration joins a series of elements that are not linked through COORD. Each
element is linked to the first item in the series which serves as the head. The series could
Part 3: Morphosyntax 67

have any length, as occurs when children recite the letters of the alphabet or count to 20.
In some cases the series will end with a coordinator and final element, but the items
before the coordinator are still in an enumerated series, as in we laughed, schmoozed, had
a great time.
Here is an example of a series of two items (helpful, considerate) with a final
coordination (and professional):

*PAR: Ron is helpful , considerate , and professional .


%mor: n:prop|Ron cop|be&3S adj|helpful cm|cm adj|considerate cm|cm conj|and
adj|professional .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|2|PRED 4|3|LP 5|3|ENUM 6|5|LP 7|3|CONJ 8|7|COORD
9|2|PUNCT

ENUM can also bind together more complex phrases as in the following example where
it binds together neither the pink one and nor the blue one into one constituent headed by
[pink] one, which can then act as the SUBJ of the sentence. Each of neither and nor is
tied to its corresponding item as a COORD, and then [blue] one is tied to [pink] one as
an ENUM.

*PAR: neither the pink one nor the blue one fits well .
%mor: conj|neither art|the adj|pink n|one conj|nor art|the adj|blue n|one v|fit-3S adv|well .
%gra: 1|4|COORD 2|4|DET 3|4|MOD 4|9|SUBJ 5|8|COORD 6|8|DET 7|8|DET 8|4|ENUM
9|0|ROOT 10|9|JCT 11|9|PUNCT

Here is another example in which the entire phrase as quiet as a mouse is bound into one
constituent, with the PRED quiet as its head. This is accomplished by tying each as to its
corresponding item as a COORD, and then tying mouse to quiet as an ENUM.

*PAR: be as quiet as a mouse .


%mor: v|be prep|as adj|quiet prep|as art|a n|mouse .
Part 3: Morphosyntax 68

%gra: 1|0|ROOT 2|3|COORD 3|1|PRED 4|6|COORD 5|6|DET 6|3|ENUM 7|1|PUNCT

11.10 POSTMOD
POSTMODifier identifies a postposed nominal modifier, which might be either an
adjective or another noun. POSTMOD can express resultative relations (I painted the
barn red; let’s keep it a private affair) and can also serve when a modifying phrase
gravitates toward the end of the phrase due to size (he dug a hole wider than a pickup
truck). There may be intervening content in the sentence (she made it out of the haunted
house alive, where alive is a POSTMOD to she). As with MOD, the head of POSTMOD
is the noun it modifies.

*PAR: I've never met anyone smarter .


%mor: pro:sub|I~aux|have adv|never part|meet&PASTP pro:indef|anyone
adj|smart-CP .
%gra: 1|4|SUBJ 2|4|AUX 3|4|JCT 4|0|ROOT 5|4|OBJ 6|5|POSTMOD 7|4|PUNCT

This is a very basic instance of POSTMOD, where smart modifies anyone but is
postposed.

*PAR: you're kind_of making me nervous .


%mor: pro:sub|you~aux|be&PRES adv|kind_of part|make-PRESP pro:obj|me
adj|nervous .
%gra: 1|4|SUBJ 2|4|AUX 3|4|JCT 4|0|ROOT 5|4|OBJ 6|5|POSTMOD 7|4|PUNCT

making me nervous exhibits a resultative relationship. me ties as an OBJ to making, while


nervous, the resultative complement, ties to me as a POSTMOD. Other verbs like get,
keep, and put can participate in similar constructions (e.g. get the crowd excited, keep it
secret, put the blazer on).
Part 3: Morphosyntax 69

*PAR: she burned the whole boat , oars and all .


%mor: pro:sub|she v|burn-PAST art|the adj|whole n|boat cm|cm n|oar-PL conj|and pro:indef|all .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|5|DET 4|5|MOD 5|2|OBJ 6|5|LP 7|5|POSTMOD 8|7|CONJ
9|8|COORD 10|2|PUNCT

Here we see a detached phrase, oars and all, serving to add information to boat yet not
explicitly connected to it by a verb or preposition. We’ll solve this by tying oar to boat as
a POSTMOD. and all then connect to oar using standard CONJ-COORD relations.
The trickiest question is where to tie the comma. Strictly speaking, this is not a list, but
the whole boat, oars and all still gives the impression of being one large constituent in
the sentence, so tying the comma all the way back to the root (as one would if the comma
were separating distinct clauses) is unsatisfactory. Instead treat it as you would a list,
tying the comma to the preceding boat, to preserve the constituency of the whole boat,
oars and all.
POSTMOD can also be used in sentences such as the woman leapt up, hat askew. Here
hat askew creates an unusual problem since it appears syntactically to belong to the
sentence, yet it is not explicitly tied in with a preposition or a verb. Use the POSTMOD
category to tie the “loose” NP to the preceding NP it describes. So hat should tie to
woman as a POSTMOD. Don’t confuse this with the appositive APP, where two NPs act
as alternating names for the same entity.

11.11 COMP, LINK


COMPlement identifies a finite clausal complement to a verb, such as a subordinate
clause introduced by a verb like say, know, or think (they know we’re here). The main
verb of this subordinate clause, being its topmost element, is the item that should be
classed as COMP. The head of COMP is the verb for which the clause is acting as a
complement. Here is an example of COMP without LINK:

*PAR: I can't believe you helped him .


%mor: pro:sub|I mod|can~neg|not v|believe pro:subj|you v|help-PAST pro:obj|him .
%gra: 1|4|SUBJ 2|4|AUX 3|2|NEG 4|0|ROOT 5|6|SUBJ 6|4|COMP 7|6|OBJ 8|4|PUNCT

LINK identifies a word in a number of categories that can introduce a subordinate clause,
such as complementizers (that in I heard that you’d given it up), relativizers (who in the
girl who told me about the concert), and subordinate conjunctions (unless in we’re fine
unless the power goes out). One of the uses of LINK is to include the complementizer in
Part 3: Morphosyntax 70

a COMP clause, as in this example, which adds that to the previous example:

*PAR: I can't believe that you helped him .


%mor: pro:sub|I mod|can~neg|not v|believe rel|that pro:subj|you v|help-PAST pro:obj|him .
%gra: 1|4|SUBJ 2|4|AUX 3|2|NEG 4|0|ROOT 5|7|LINK 6|7|SUBJ 7|4|COMP 8|7|OBJ
9|4|PUNCT

LINK is also used to join a subordinating conjunction to the verb of a subordinate clause.
*** example missing ***

Sometimes such clauses can appear without main clauses. For example, an English
speaker may say unless the power goes out on its own, in which case unless is still
classed as a LINK. This is especially common with sentences beginning with the
coordinators and, but, or, etc. In all these cases, the head of LINK is the main verb of its
clause.

11.12 QUANT and PQ


QUANTifier identifies a nominal quantifier, such as many or several. Numbers that
quantify nouns also fall into this category (three foxes), as do distributive terms like each
and any. However, in all these cases, the QUANT must be modifying a noun phrase; if
there is no noun phrase being modified, then the quantifier is presumably acting as a
pronoun and should be classified as such. (For example, in some say he’s a traitor, some
is the SUBJ, not a QUANT.)
Like MODs, QUANTs do not stack when they quantify the same noun phrase, nor do
they interact with MODs or a DET. All tie individually to the head noun. The head of
QUANT is the noun it modifies.
Here is an example with two cases of QUANTs modifying noun phrases: two and
some. Just like DET, QUANT doesn’t interact with MOD at all, but instead ties
separately to the noun it quantifies, while MOD does the same. So two ties to onions
here, not yellow.

*PAR: could you grab me two yellow onions and some carrots ?
%mor: mod|could pro:sub|you v|grab pro:obj|me det:num|two adj|yellow n|onion-PL conj|and
qn|some n|carrot-PL ?
%gra: 1|3|AUX 2|3|SUBJ 3|0|ROOT 4|3|OBJ2 5|7|QUANT 6|7|MOD 7|3|OBJ 8|7|CONJ
9|10|QUANT 10|8|COORD 11|3|PUNCT
Part 3: Morphosyntax 71

Here is an example with two QUANTs and MOD linked to a single head:

*PAR: Maisie picked up some more new skills .


%mor: n:prop|Maisie v|pick-PAST prep|up qn|some qn|more adj|new n|skill-PL .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|2|JCT 4|7|QUANT 5|7|QUANT 6|7|MOD 7|3|POBJ 8|2|PUNCT

PostQuantifier = PQ identifies a quantifier that follows rather than precedes its noun
phrase. This mostly occurs with both (we both tried it) and all (they all gave up). As with
POSTMOD, it’s acceptable for some sentential content to intervene between the
quantified noun and the PQ. The head of PQ is the noun it modifies.

*PAR: you should really both try to apologize .


%mor: pro:sub|you mod|should adv|real&dadj-LY qn|both v|try inf|to v|apologize .
%gra: 1|5|SUBJ 2|5|AUX 3|5|JCT 4|1|PQ 5|0|ROOT 6|7|INF 7|5|COMP 8|5|PUNCT

It is not a problem that should and really intervene between you and both here. both still
ties to you as a PQ, since that’s the term it serves to quantify.

11.13 CSUBJ, COBJ, CPOBJ, CPRED


Clausal SUBJect = CSUBJ, Clausal OBJect = COBJ, Clausal Prepositional OBJect
= CPOBJ, and Clausal PRED = CPRED are variations on the categories SUBJ, OBJ,
POBJ, and PRED. In each of these cases, the appropriate syntactic role is being
performed by an entire verbal clause (which could be either finite or non-finite) rather
than a noun phrase.

CSUBJ: The main verb of that clause, being the topmost element, is the one that will be
identified as CSUBJ, etc. From there, the clause ties to the rest of the sentence exactly as
its non-clausal equivalent would. So, the head of CSUBJ, for example, is the verb for
which that clause acts as the subject. In this next example, the entire clause Leroy getting
killed off is the subject which is ascribed the quality of being a terrible ending. Thus the
main verb of that clause, killed, is tied as a CSUBJ to was.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 72

*PAR: Leroy getting killed off was a terrible ending.


%mor: n:prop|Leroy part|get-PRESP part|kill-PASTP prep|off cop|be&PAST&13S art|a
adj|terrible n|ending .
%gra: 1|3|SUBJ 2|3|AUX 3|5|CSUBJ 4|3|JCT 5|0|ROOT 6|8|DET 7|8|MOD 8|5|PRED
9|5|PUNCT

COBJ: In this next example, whoever keeps calling me is a clause here acting as the
object of want. So the main verb of this clause, keeps, ties to want as a COBJ.

*PAR: I want whoever keeps calling me to stop .


%mor: pro:sub|I v|want pro:wh|whoever v|keep-3S part|call-PRESP pro:obj|me inf|to v|stop .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|4|SUBJ 4|2|COBJ 5|4|COMP 6|5|OBJ 7|8|INF 8|2|COMP
9|2|PUNCT

CPOBJ: In this example, the clause why she did that acts as the object of the preposition
on; thus its main verb did ties to on as a CPOBJ:

*PAR: I'm not clear on why she did that .


%mor: pro:sub|I~cop|be&1S neg|not adj|clear prep|on adv:wh|why pro:sub|she v|do-PAST
pro:dem|that .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|2|NEG 4|2|PRED 5|4|JCT 6|8|LINK 7|8|SUBJ 8|5|CPOBJ 9|8|OBJ
10|2|PUNCT

CPRED: In this example, getting the coin to land just right is the predicate to the main
verb be. Thus, its main verb getting ties to be as a CPRED.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 73

*PAR: the trick is getting the coin to land just right .


%mor: art|the n|trick cop|be&3S part|get-PRESP art|the n|coin inf|to v|land adv|just adv|right .
%gra: 1|2|DET 2|3|SUBJ 3|0|ROOT 4|3|PRED 5|6|DET 6|4|OBJ 7|8|INF 8|4|COMP 9|10|JCT
10|8|JCT 11|3|PUNCT

11.14 CJCT and XJCT


Clausal conJunCT = CJCT identifies a finite clause that functions as an adjunct to a
verb, adjective, or adverb. The most common attachment is for a subordinate clause
attached to a main clause. For example, in the sentence you’ll do it, because I said so, the
subordinate clause because I said so is a clausal conjunct on the verbof the main clause
do.As with the clausal categories introduced in the previous section, the main verb within
the adjoining clause (said in the above example) is the item that should be tagged CJCT,
and its head is the main verb of the clause to which it adjoins (do). The conjunction
because is linked to said through the LINK relation. Here is another example in which
until we have brought you to the infirmary is a finite clausal adjunct, and so its main verb
brought ties to try as a CJCT, and until functions as a LINK introducing the adjoining
clause.

*PAR: try not to move until we've brought you to the infirmary .
%mor: v|try neg|not inf|to v|move conj|until pro:sub|we~aux|have part|brought-PASTP
pro:obj|you prep|to art|the n|infirmary .
%gra: 1|0|ROOT 2|4|NEG 3|4|INF 4|1|COMP 5|8|LINK 6|8|SUBJ 7|8|AUX 8|1|CJCT 9|8|OBJ
10|8|JCT 11|12|DET 12|10|POBJ 13|1|PUNCT

XadJunCT = XJCT identifies a non-finite clause (that is, generally, one headed by an
infinitive verb or participle) that otherwise behaves like a CJCT, as in to build a ship, first
gather some wood. Again, the main verb in the adjoining clause (build in the example) is
tagged as XJCT and its head is the main verb of the clause to which it adjoins (gather).
In the following example, visiting museums and galleries is a non-finite clausal adjunct
whose main verb ties to spend as an XJCT.

*PAR: we spent the whole day visiting museums and galleries .


%mor: pro:sub|we v|spend-PAST art|the adj|whole n|day part|visit-PRESP n|museum-PL
Part 3: Morphosyntax 74

conj|and n|gallery-PL .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|5|DET 4|5|MOD 5|2|OBJ 6|2|XJCT 7|6|OBJ 8|7|CONJ 9|8|COORD
10|2|PUNCT

11.15 CMOD and XMOD


Clausal MODifier = CMOD identifies a finite clause that modifies a noun, as in the boy
(that) Tricia took to prom. CMOD also sometimes modifies an adjective or adverb, if that
adjective/adverb takes a complement (we’re happy that you made it) or participates in a
resultative expression (so quietly I couldn’t hear them). CMOD functions much like
CJCT and XJCT: the main verb within the modifier clause is the item tagged CMOD, and
its head is the noun, adjective, or adverb it modifies.
In the following example, the clausal adjunct I wore for the race does not modify the
main verb throwing, but rather shoes – this clause specifies a particular pair of the
speaker’s shoes. Therefore wore ties to shoes as a CMOD rather than tying to throwing as
a CJCT.

*PAR: I'm throwing away the shoes I wore for the race .
%mor: pro:sub|I~aux|be&1S part|throw-PRESP prep|away art|the n|shoe-PL pro:sub|I v|wear-
PAST prep|for art|the n|race .
%gra: 1|3|SUBJ 2|3|AUX 3|0|ROOT 4|3|JCT 5|6|DET 6|4|POBJ 7|8|SUBJ 8|6|CMOD 9|8|JCT
10|11|DET 11|9|POBJ 12|3|PUNCT

*PAR: they were so hungry that they couldn't sleep .


%mor: pro:sub|they cop|be&PAST adv|so adj|hungry rel|that pro:sub|they mod|could~neg|not
v|sleep .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|4|JCT 4|2|PRED 5|9|LINK 6|9|SUBJ 7|9|AUX 8|7|NEG 9|4|CMOD
10|2|PUNCT

CMOD can also modify an adjective, as in this example:

*PAR: they were so hungry that they couldn't sleep .


%mor: pro:sub|they cop|be&PAST adv|so adj|hungry rel|that pro:sub|they mod|could~neg|not
Part 3: Morphosyntax 75

v|sleep .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|4|JCT 4|2|PRED 5|9|LINK 6|9|SUBJ 7|9|AUX 8|7|NEG 9|4|CMOD
10|2|PUNCT

XMODifier identifies a non-finite clause that otherwise behaves like a CMOD, as in the
person running the store; a good day to take a walk; she’s eager to teach them.
Once again, the main verb within the modifying clause is the one tagged XMOD, and
its head is the noun, adjective, or adverb it modifies. In this example, the head is one.

*PAR: Isabelle is the one to ask about computer problems .


%mor: n:prop|Isabelle cop|be&3S art|the n|one inf|to v|ask prep|about n|computer n|problem-PL
.
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|4|DET 4|2|PRED 5|6|INF 6|4|XMOD 7|6|JCT 8|9|MOD 9|7|POBJ
10|2|PUNCT

11.16 BEG, BEGP, END, ENDP


BEGinning identifies a clause-external element occurring at the beginning of an
utterance. This might be, for example, an interjection like hey or okay, or a vocative like
the addressee’s name.
The head of BEG is 0, the LeftWall.

BEGinning Punctuation = BEGP identifies the double dagger ‡ which is employed to


partition off a BEG element from the rest of the sentence for the purposes of MOR. The
double dagger should always follow a BEG.
The head of BEGP is the BEG it follows.

Here is an example of BEG and BEGP in which the BEG okay ties directly to 0, while
the BEGP ties to okay.

*PAR: okay ‡ here's the floor she's trying to rent out .


%mor: co|okay beg|beg pro:exist|here~cop|be&3S art|the n|floor pro:sub|she~aux|be&3S
part|try-PRESP inf|to v|rent prep|out .
%gra: 1|0|BEG 2|1|BEGP 3|4|JCT 4|0|ROOT 5|6|DET 6|4|PRED 7|9|SUBJ 8|9|AUX 9|6|CMOD
10|11|INF 11|9|COMP 12|11|JCT 13|4|PUNCT

END identifies a clause-external element occuring at the end of an utterance. This


includes interjections, vocatives, and other elements similar to those that can act as
BEGs. However, it doesn’t include tag questions like …don’t you? Whereas the head of
Part 3: Morphosyntax 76

BEG is 0, the head of END is the ROOT.

END Punctuation = ENDP identifies the double comma „ which is employed to


partition off an END element much like the double dagger. The double comma should
always precede an END or a TAG. The head of ENDP is the END it precedes.

Here is an example of END and ENDP in which the END here Hugh ties to the ROOT
want (then the ENDP ties to Hugh) and the ENDP ties to END.

*PAR: you'll probably want to keep an eye on that „ Hugh .


%mor: pro:sub|you~mod|will adv|probable&dadj-LY v|want inf|to v|keep art|an n|eye prep|on
pro:dem|that end|end n:prop|Hugh .
%gra: 1|4|SUBJ 2|4|AUX 3|4|JCT 4|0|ROOT 5|6|INF 6|4|COMP 7|8|DET 8|6|OBJ 9|8|NJCT
10|9|POBJ 11|12|ENDP 12|4|END 13|4|PUNCT

11.17 COM and TAG


COMmunicator identifies any clause-external element occuring medially rather than
initially or finally. Depending on the transcriber, such an element might be partitioned off
with commas (I walked into the courtroom, right, and here’s this guy telling me…), or it
might not be partitioned at all (I know you don’t like it Li but it’s the best solution we’ve
got). Like END, and unlike BEG, the head of COM is the ROOT.

Here is an example:

*PAR: I think it's possible , sure , but who can say ?


%mor: pro:sub|I v|think pro|it~cop|be&3S adj|possible cm|cm co|sure cm|cm conj|but pro:wh|who
mod|can v|say ?
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|4|SUBJ 4|2|COMP 5|4|PRED 6|2|LP 7|2|COM 8|2|LP 9|12|LINK
10|12|SUBJ 11|12|AUX 12|2|CJCT 13|2|PUNCT

Both of the commas used to partition off sure are also tied to the ROOT (since they’re
not delineating items in a list, but rather entire clauses).

TAG identifies the verb of a tag question at the end of a sentence (…doesn’t it?). As
mentioned in the previous section, this is not considered an END, but has a category of its
Part 3: Morphosyntax 77

own since it takes a more complex set of syntactic interactions. However, as with END,
there may be a double comma preceding the tag verb; if so, tie it to the tag verb as an
ENDP. The head of TAG, like END, is the ROOT.

Here is an example of TAG in action. The tag verb, have, ties to the ROOT toured. That
tag verb takes its own NEG not and even its own SUBJ you, so it’s more complex than an
END. However, just as if it were an END, the double comma ties to it as an ENDP.
PUNCT still ties to the ROOT, because we want to tie PUNCT consistently to the ROOT
in all cases.

*PAR: you've toured just about everywhere „ haven't you ?


%mor: pro:sub|you~aux|have part|tour-PASTP adv|just adv|about adv|everywhere end|end
aux|have~neg|not pro:sub|you ?
%gra: 1|3|SUBJ 2|3|AUX 3|0|ROOT 4|5|JCT 5|6|JCT 6|3|JCT 7|8|ENDP 8|3|TAG 9|8|NEG
10|8|SUBJ 11|3|PUNCT

11.18 SRL, APP


SeRiaL identifies serial verbs, such as come play and go see. In each of these pairs, the
second verb is treated as the head and the first verb is treated as the dependent. Thus, the
first verb is tagged SRL and its head is the second verb.
Generally serial verb pairs are both bare verbs, but there’s at least one exception: get in
the sense of get going or another get X-ing. Notice that the second verb doesn’t seem to
relate to get as an object or complement, so the function of get appears more serial.
(Contrast the more transparent start packing, try spinning, etc., where the second verb
would tie as an COMP to the first verb.) Here is an example in which go ties to check as
a SRL.

*PAR: go check whether the rice is ready .


%mor: v|go v|check conj|whether art|the n|rice cop|be&3S adj|ready .
%gra: 1|2|SRL 2|0|ROOT 3|6|LINK 4|5|DET 5|6|SUBJ 6|2|COMP 7|6|PRED 8|2|PUNCT

APPositive identifies an appositive noun or phrase, as in Giles the baker. The head of
APP is the noun it corresponds to. However, which of the two apposed terms should be
considered the head and which the APP will depend on the structure of the sentence.
In most straightforward constructions (as in the one above), the first term is the head
Part 3: Morphosyntax 78

and the following term is the APP (so baker will tie to Giles as an APP). On the other
hand, a sentence might be structured such that the second term – especially if it’s a
pronoun – will read more like the SUBJ (etc.) and the first term will read more like the
APP. In the sentence myself, I prefer provolone, treat I as the SUBJ and tie myself to it as
an APP.
Remember the essential quality of APP: both noun phrases denote the same entity, and
serve as two different ways of naming it. That makes APP unlike our hat askew example
from back in section 9, which is why APP is inappropriate there, and POSTMOD is used
instead.

*PAR: he told his boss , Masha , he was switching to a new job .


%mor: pro:sub|he v|tell-PAST pro:poss:det|his n|boss cm|cm n:prop|Masha cm|cm pro:sub|he
aux|be&PAST&13S part|switch-PRESP prep|to art|a adj|new n|job .
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|4|MOD 4|2|OBJ 5|4|LP 6|4|APP 7|6|LP 8|10|SUBJ 9|10|AUX
10|2|COMP 11|10|JCT 12|14|DET 13|14|MOD 14|11|NJCT 15|2|PUNCT

Here Masha ties to boss as an APP. Notice that the transcriber put commas here to
partition Masha from the rest of the sentence. This is optional – it makes no difference in
how Masha ties to boss – but take note of how the commas are treated, each one tied to
the word preceding. Essentially the commas behave exactly as if this were a list of items
conjoined with ENUM (so they tie directly to those items, rather than the ROOT).

11.19 NAME, DATE


NAME identifies relations within a string of elements that are functioning together to act
as one name, like Deandre Clarks and South Carolina. When transcribing, these should
generally be conjoined with underscores to form single syntactic units (Deandre_Clarks
and South_Carolina). However, if the two or more elements of a name are not or cannot
be conjoined (for instance, if there’s intervening material: Deandre &uh Clarks), the
NAME tag will be required.
Use NAME to tie every non-final element in the name to the final element. Thus in
South Carolina, South will tie to Carolina as a NAME.

DATE identifies relations within a string of elements that are functioning together to act
as a date, like July twentieth nineteen ninety. Unlike proper names, these are never
conjoined with underscores to form single syntactic units, since the combination of
numbers would be endless. Thus DATE is required in all cases.
For consistency with NAME, tie every non-final element in the date to the final
element. Thus in July twentieth nineteen ninety, all elements but ninety will tie to it as
DATEs.
\
Part 3: Morphosyntax 79

*PAR: June sixteenth , nineteen oh [: zero] four was just another day for Leopold Bloom .
%mor: n:prop|June adj|sixteenth cm|cm det:num|nineteen det:num|zero det:num|four
cop|be&PAST&13S adv|just qn|another n|day prep|for n:prop|Leopold n:prop|Bloom .
%gra: 1|6|DATE 2|6|DATE 3|2|LP 4|6|DATE 5|6|DATE 6|7|SUBJ 7|0|ROOT 8|9|JCT
9|10|QUANT 10|7|PRED 11|10|NJCT 12|13|NAME 13|11|POBJ 14|7|PUNCT

11.20 INCROOT, OM
INCROOT is a catch-all category for any word that must substitute in the role of ROOT
when no proper ROOT is available. This may be because the sentence lacks a verb
altogether, or because any verb present is not top-level (say, it’s part of a clause
depending on another phrase). When no ROOT is available, choose the topmost word in
the sentence and name it the INCROOT. The head of INCROOT is 0. Here is an example
of INCROOT in action. Since there’s no verb in this sentence, there is no proper ROOT,
and we must assign the label of INCROOT to another element in the sentence. The
topmost existing element is one, upon which the determiner the depends, as well as the
entire NJCT phrase that follows. Thus, it is the logical choice to be identified as the
INCROOT. Naturally, PUNCT will now tie to one as if it were the ROOT.

*PAR: the one with the stripes ?


%mor: art|the n|one prep|with art|the n|stripe-PL ?
%gra: 1|2|DET 2|0|INCROOT 3|2|NJCT 4|5|DET 5|3|POBJ 6|2|PUNCT

OMission is another catch-all for any determiner, modifier, or other dependent word
which is left headless due to an omission, such as the speaker trailing off or being
interrupted and hence leaving their sentence unfinished. The head of OM is whatever the
head of its head would have been. (So the head of OM is the ROOT, for instance, if
what’s been omitted was the OBJ of the root verb.)

*PAR: Eloise wrote the +...


%mor: n:prop|Eloise v|write-PAST art|the +...
%gra: 1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|2|OM 4|2|PUNCT
Part 3: Morphosyntax 80

12 GRs for other languages


Although many of the basic GRs for English can be found in other languages, there are
often relations in these other languages that are not found in English.

12.1 Spanish
The GRs needed for Spanish are close to those for English. The distinction between X
and C in the XJCT/CJCT and XMOD/CMOD relations are irrelevant to Spanish because
the finite/nonfinite status of the verb is clearly reflected in its morphology. So, the XJCT
and XMOD relations can be dropped. The two additional relations that are needed are
1. APREP for the a-personal. This relation can either mark the direct object, when
no OBJ is present or the indirect object when there is an OBJ relation. The APREP
attaches to the verb and the object of the preposition attaches to the “a” with the
POBJ relation.
2. INF for the preposition introducing an infinitive. For example, in lo hizo para
salir, the preposition para relates to the infinitive salir with the INF relation. The
infinitive then relates to the main verb with a COMP relation.

12.2 Chinese
The GRs needed for Chinese are not too very different from those needed for English.
The major differences involve these areas:
1. Chinese uses all the basic GRs that are also found in English with the exception of
INF.

2. Also, Chinese does not have a finite, non-finite distinction on verbs. This makes the
“X” relations of English irrelevant and only CSUBJ, COMP, CMOD, PRED, and
CJCT are needed. The main verb is the head of the subordinate clause.

3. Chinese often combines clauses without using any conjunction to mark


subordination or coordination. In this case, it is best to transcribe the two clauses as
separate sentences. To mark the missing subordination relation, just add this
postcode to the end of the first sentence: [+ sub]. This mark does not necessarily
imply which clause is subordinate; it just notes that the two clauses are related,
although the relation is not marked with a conjunction.

4. SFP is a relation between the sentence final particle and “0”.

5. SRL is for serial verb constructions. For Chinese, unlike English, the head of SRL
is the first verb, not the second.

6. POSS in Chinese is the relation between the linker “de” and the preceding possessor
noun or pronoun which then functions as the head for further attachment to the thing
possessed through the MOD relation.

7. JCT can be used in Chinese for classifier phrases that such as yi1 bian4.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 81

8. JCT also extends also to the complements of directional verbs, as in this example:
他 跑 进 瓶子 里面.
pro|ta1=he v|pao3=run v:dirc|jin4=enter n|ping2-NOM=bottle
post|li3mian4=inside.
1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|2|SRL 4|5|POSTO 5|2|JCT 6|2|PUNCT
Note that the JCT is attached to the first of the two serial verbs which is the head of
the serial verb pair.

9. CJCT is used in cases where there are no subordinating conjunctions, as in this


example:
青蛙 一 看 就 没 了 .
n|qing1wa1=frog adv|yi1=just_as v|kan4=look adv|jiu4=just
neg|mei2=not asp|le .
1|6|COM 2|4|SUBJ 3|4|JCT 4|0|ROOT 5|6|JCT 6|4|PRED 7|6|CLEFT
17 8|4|PUNCT
It can also extend to linking the phrase mei2 you3 to the main verb.
没 有 讲 完.
neg|mei2=not v|you3=have v|jiang3=speak v:resc|wan2=finish.
1|2|NEG 2|3|CJCT 3|0|ROOT 4|3|SRL 5|3|PUNCT

10. PRED can be used to code clefts, as in this example:


唻 姐姐 就 是 这样 讲 的 .
co|lai2 n:relat|jie3&DIM=elder_sister adv|jiu4=just v:cop|shi4=is
pro|zhe4yang4=so_such v|jiang3=speak nom|de .
1|6|COM 2|6|SUBJ 3|4|JCT 4|6|JCT 5|6|SUBJ 6|0|ROOT 7|6|POSS
8|6|PUNCT

11. TOP is the basic relation used in Chinese for verbless sentences and sentences with
resumptive pronouns. In the case of verbless sentences, the ROOT of the clause is
the predicate adjective or noun and the initial topic attaches to that element with the
TOP relation, as in this example:
物价 纽约 最 贵 .
n|wu4jia4=commodity n-prop|niu3yue1=New_York adv|zui4=the_most
adj|gui4=expensive .
1|4|TOP 2|4|SUBJ 3|4|JCT 4|0|INCROOT 5|4|PUNCT

In the case of resumptive pronouns, the initial topic attaches to the resumptive
pronoun with the TOP relation

12. PTOP is a postposed topic, as in this example:


这 个 是 什么 这 个 .
pro|zhe4=this cl|ge4 v:cop|shi4=is pro:wh|shen2me=what
pro|zhe4=this cl|ge4 .
1|2|DET 2|3|SUBJ 3|0|ROOT 4|3|PRED 5|6|DET 6|3|PTOP 7|3|PUNCT

13. POSTO is the relation between a noun and a following postposition. The
postposition is the head and the nominal object of the preposition is the dependent.
The postposition is linked to the verb as JCT. The relation is marked on the noun, as
Part 3: Morphosyntax 82

in this example:
他 跑 到 教室 里面 去 .
pro|ta1=he v|pao3=run v:resc|dao4=arrive v|jiao1shi4=classroom
post|li3mian4=inside v:dirc|qu4=go .
1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|2|SRL 4|5|POSTO 5|2|JCT 6|2|SRL 7|2|PUNCT

14. PREPO is the relation between a preposition and a following noun. The preposition
is the head and the object of the preposition is the dependent. The preposition is
linked to the verb as JCT, as in this example:
他 是 从 北京 来 的 .
pro|ta1=he v:cop|shi4=is prep|cong2=from n:geo|bei3jing1=Beijing
v|lai2=come nom|de .
1|2|SUBJ 2|0|ROOT 3|2|JCT 4|3|PREPO 5|2|CMOD 6|2|LINK 7|2|PUNCT

PREPO and PREP can be combined, as in this example:


养 在 什么 里面 ?
v|yang3=raise prep|zai4=at pro:wh|shen2me=what post|li3mian4=insid?
1|0|ROOT 2|1|JCT 3|2|PREPO 4|2|POSTO 5|1|PUNCT

12.3 Japanese
The GRs needed for Japanese are more differentiated than those needed for English. The
major differences involve these areas:
1. Japanese is left-branched with the head following the modifier, adjunct or
complement.
2. Japanese uses optional case particles to identify case relations. The head noun is
coded as SUBJ or OBJ, resp. The case particles are coded as CASP, and constitute
the head of the argument. Free adjuncts are coded as JCT, and the corresponding
postparticles as POSTP.
Ken ga Tookyoo kara kita
Ken SUBJ Tokyo from come-PAST “Ken came from Tokyo”
1|2|SUBJ 2|5|CASP 3|4|JCT 4|5|POSTP 5|0|ROOT

3. The root can be a tense-bearing element like a verb, a verbal adjective (ROOT), a
copula (COPROOT) or a noun (PREDROOT).
4. The root can be also a case particle (CASPROOT), a postparticle (POSTPROOT),
an attributive particle (ATTPROOT), a quotative particle (QUOTPROOT), a topic
particle (TOPPROOT), a quotative marker, a focus particle (FOCPROOT), or a
conjunctive particle (CPZRROOT). Note that these different types of roots are fully
grammatical and not elliptic fragments.
Papa no.
Dad GEN “(it's) Dad's one.”
1|2|MOD 2|0|ATTPROOT
iku kara.
go-PRES because “because (I) will go”
1|2|COMP 2|0|CPZRROOT

5. Japanese expresses topic relations (TOP); the topic particle is coded as TOPP.
6. Like Chinese, Japanese has no articles and uses classifiers and counters to mark
quantification.
Part 3: Morphosyntax 83

sankurambo sanko tabeta.


cherry 3-pieces eat-PAST “he ate 3 cherries”
1|3| OBJ 2|1|QUANT 3|0|ROOT

7. Spoken Japanese makes extensive use of sentence final particles (SFP) and sentence
modifiers (SMDR). They are depending of the preceding ROOT.
tabeta no ?
eat-PAST SFP “did you eat (it)?”
1|0|ROOT 2|1|SFP
tabeta jan.
eat-PAST SMDR “you ate it, didn't you?”
1|0|ROOT 2|1|SMDR

Root ROOT verbal ROOT; relation between verb and left wall: v,
adj, subsidiary verb (tense bearing element)
taberu. ‘I’ll eat it.’
1|0|ROOT
COPROOT COPula ROOT; copula with noun, adjectival noun, or
sentence nominalizer (no da)
koko da. ‘it’s here.’
1|2|PRED 2|0|COPROOT
PREDROOT nominal ROOT (without copula); includes adv, co,
and quant, as well as verbal nouns and adjectival
nouns in root position.
koko. ‘it’s here.’
1|0|PREDROOT
CPREDROOT nominal ROOT with a sentence nominalizer in root
position (ptl:snr|no=SNR)
uma no chiisai no. ‘a small horse’
1|2|MOD 2|3|CASP 3|4|CMOD 4|0|CPREDROOT
CASPROOT CASe Particle ROOT (the case particle is the head)
dare ga? ‘who?’
1|2|SUBJ 2|0|CASPROOT

TOPPROOT TOPic Particle ROOT


kore wa? ‘and what about this one?’
1|2|TOP 2|0|TOPPROOT

FOCROOT FOCus Particle ROOT


kore mo? ‘this one, too?’
1|2|FOC 2|0|FOCPROOT

Topic TOP TOPicalization, (for convenience the root of the


sentence is considered to be the head)
kore wa yomenai. ‘I can’t read it.’
1|2|TOP 2|3|TOPP 3|0|ROOT
Part 3: Morphosyntax 84

CTOP finite Clausal TOPic (head of the clause is ptl:snr|no)


iku no wa ii kedo [...] ‘it’s ok to go, but…’
1|2|CMOD 2|3|CTOP 3|4|TOPP 4|5|COMP 5|6|CPZR
FOC FOCus (followed by ptl:foc; mo, shika, bakari, hodo
etc.)
kore mo yonda. ‘he read this one, too.’
1|2|FOC 2|3|FOCP 3|0|ROOT
Arguments SUBJ nonclausal SUBject
Jon ga tabeta. ‘John ate it.’
1|2|SUBJ 2|3|CASP 3|0|ROOT
CSUBJ finite Clausal SUBJect (head of the clause is ptl:snr)
taberu no ga ii. ‘it’s good to eat it.’
1|2|CMOD 2|3|CSUBJ 3|4|CASP 3|0|ROOT
OBJ accusative OBJect
hon o yonda. ‘he has read the book.’
1|2|OBJ 2|3|CASP 3|0|ROOT
COBJ finite Clausal accusative OBJect
taberu no o yameta. ‘he stopped eating.’
1|2|CMOD 2|3|COBJ 3|4|CASP 4|0|ROOT
Adjuncts JCT adJunCT (Postpositional or adverbial phrase)
gakkoo kara kaetta. ‘he came back from school.’
1|2|JCT 2|3|POSTP 3|0|ROOT
yukkuri shabetta. ‘he talked slowly.’
1|2|JCT 2|0|ROOT
CJCT finite Clausal adJunCT
ochita no de taberu. ‘I’ll eat with the one that had
fallen down.
1|2|CMOD 2|3|CJCT 3|4|POSTP 4|0|ROOT
XJCT nonfinite clause as adJunCT (tabe-reba, -tara, -te, -
cha, -tari; oishi-ku; shizuka ni)
kaeseba ii. ‘it’s ok to give it back.’
1|2|XJCT 2|0|ROOT
Clause CPZR ComPlementiZeR (subordinating conjunctive
conjunction particle; ptl:conj|)
osoi kara kaeru. ‘I’ll go home because it’s late.’
1|2|COMP 2|3|CPZR 3|0|ROOT
ZCPZR Zero-ComPlementiZeR (sentence introducing
conjunction); head is always the root
dakara kaeru. ‘that’s why I’ll go home.’
1|2|ZCPZR 2|0|ROOT
COMP finite clausal verb COMPlement (before ptl:conj| and
quot|to )
osoi kara kaeru. ‘I’ll go home because it’s late.’
1|2|COMP 2|3|CPZR 3|0|ROOT
QUOTP QUOTation Particle after nominal or verbal phrase
kaeru to iimashita. ‘he said he would go home.’
Part 3: Morphosyntax 85

13COMP 21QUOTP 30ROOT

ZQUOT Zero-QUOTative (sentence introducing quotative


marker)
tte iu ka […] ‘in other words’
1|2|ZQUOT 2|3|COMP 3|4|CPZR
Nominal MOD nonclausal MODifier (of a nominal)
head Papa no kutsu ga atta. ‘there are Dad’s shoes.’
1|2|MOD 2|3|CASP 3|4|SUBJ 4|5|CASP 5|0|ROOT
CMOD finite Clausal MODifier of a nominal; the dependent
is a finite verb, adjective or adj noun with copula
akai kuruma o mita. ‘he saw a red car.’
1|2|CMOD 2|3|OBJ 3|4|CASP 4|0|ROOT
XMOD nonfinite clausal MODifier of a nominal (adn|)
kore to onaji mono ga […] ‘a thing similar to this
one’
1|2|JCT 2|3|POSTP 3|4|XMOD 4|5|SUBJ 5|6|CASP
COORD COORDination, second noun is the head; (ptl:coo|)
inu to neko o katte iru. ‘he has a dog and a cat.’
1|2|COORD 2|3|COOP 3|4|OBJ 4|5|CASP 5|6|XJCT
6|0|ROOT
NP structure PRED nominal PREDicate before copula or QUOT
tabeta hito da. ‘he is the one who ate it.’
1|2|CMOD 2|3|PRED 3|0|COPROOT
CPRED finite Clausal PREDicate before copula (no da)
taberu no da. ‘in fact, he’ll eat it.’
1|2|CMOD 2|3|CPRED 3|0|COPROOT
CASP CASe Particles (ptl:case; ga, o)
hon o yonda. ‘he read the book.’
1|2|OBJ 2|3|CASP 3|0|ROOT
POSTP POSTpositional Particles (ptl:post; ni, de, kara,
made, to)
Papa ni ageta. ‘he gave it to Dad.’
1|2|JCT 2|3|POSTP 3|0|ROOT
ATTP ATTributive Particle
Papa no kutsu (ga) ‘Dad’s shoes are…’
1|2|MOD 2|3|ATTP 3|4|SUBJ
ATTP-SUBJ ATTributive Particle in SUBJect position with head-
noun elided
Papa no ga atta. ‘here is Dad’s one.’
1|2|MOD 2|3|ATTP-SUBJ 3|4|CASP 4|0|ROOT
ATTP-OBJ ATTributive Particle in OBJect position
Papa no o mita. ‘I saw Dad’s one.’
1|2|MOD 2|3|ATTP-OBJ 3|4|CASP 4|0|ROOT
ATTP -JCT ATTributive Particle in ADJunct position
Papa no de asonda. ‘he played with Dad’s one.’
Part 3: Morphosyntax 86

1|2|MOD 2|3|ATTP-JCT 3|4|POSTP 4|0|ROOT


ATTP -PRED ATTributive Particle in predicate position
Papa no da. ‘it’s Dad’s one.’
1|2|MOD 2|3|ATTP-PRED 3|0|COPROOT
ATTP-TOP ATTributive Particle in TOPic position
Papa no wa agenai. ‘I won’t give you Dad’s one.’
1|2|MOD 2|3|ATTP-TOP 3|4|TOPP 4|0|ROOT
ATTP-FOC ATTributive Particle in FOCus position
Papa no mo agenai. ‘I also won’t give you Dad’s
one.’
1|2|MOD 2|3|ATTP-FOC 3|4|TOPP 4|0|ROOT
TOPP TOPic Particle (ptl:top; wa)
kore wa yomenai. ‘I can’t read this.’
1|2|TOP 2|3|TOPP 3|0|ROOT
FOCP FOCus Particle (ptl:foc; mo, shika, bakari, hodo etc.)
kore mo yonda. ‘I read this one, too.’
1|2|FOC 2|3|FOCP 3|0|ROOT
COOP COOrdination Particles (ptl:coo; to, ya etc.)
inu to neko ga […] ‘dogs and cats are…’
1|2|COORD 2|3|COOP 3|4|SUBJ 4|5|CASP
QUANT QUANTifier (incl. classifiers and counters)
banana sambon tabeta. ‘he ate three bananas.’
1|3|OBJ 2|3|QUANT 3|0|ROOT
ENUM ENUMeration, without coordinating particle
ichi ni sanko da. ‘there are 1, 2, 3 of them.’
1|2|ENUM 2|3|ENUM 3|4|PRED 4|0|ROOT
NAME string of proper NAMEs, second name is the head
Kameda Taishoo ga kita. ‘Taishoo Kameda arrived.’
1|2|NAME 2|3|SUBJ 3|4|CASP 4|0|ROOT
DATE string of DATEs, last element (day) is the head
rokugatsu tsuitachi ni kita. ‘he came on June 1st.’
1|2|DATE 2|3|JCT 3|4|POSTP 4|0|ROOT
Others SMDR sentence final Sentence MoDifieR (smod| ; mitai, jan,
rashii etc); for convenience, the tense bearing verb is
considered to be the head
kaetta mitai. ‘it seems he went home.’
1|0|ROOT 2|1|SMDR
SFP Sentence Final Particle (including the use after
arguments and adjunct)
kuru ne. ‘he’ll come, won’t he?’
1|0|ROOT 2|1|SFP
Part 3: Morphosyntax 87

COM COMmunicator; (co:i| co:g|) including isolated final


particles, sentence modalizers and onomatopoeias;
head is always set to 0
anoo tabeta. ‘err..I ate it.’
1|0|COM 2|0|ROOT

VOC VOCative ; head is always set to 0


Taishoo ‡ aka. ‘Taishoo, it’s red.’
1|0|VOC 2|1|VOCP 3|0|PREDROOT
Punctuation PUNCT sentence boundary (sentence ends; .!? etc.); the root
is the head
iku . ‘I’ll go.’
1|0|ROOT 2|1|PUNCT
RDP Right Dislocation boundary (dloc|„=DISLOC);
dislocation follows; the root is the head
mita „ fuusen ? ‘the balloon, did you see it?’
1|0|ROOT 2|1|RDP 3|1|OBJ 4|1|PUNCT
VOCP VOCative marker (voc|‡=VOC ); head is the
preceding vocative
Taishoo ‡ mite ! ‘Taishoo, look!’
1|0|VOC 2|1|VOCP 3|0|ROOT 4|3|PUNCT

Berlitz. (2005). Berlitz Italian verbs handbook: 2nd Edition. New York, NY: Berlitz.
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: The use of single word utterances. The Hague:
Mouton.
Bybee, J., & Hopper, P. (2001). Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure.
Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Crain, S. (1991). Language acquisition in the absence of experience. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences, 14, 597-611.
Edwards, J. (1992). Computer methods in child language research: four principles for the
use of archived data. Journal of Child Language, 19, 435-458.
Elman, J. L. (1993). Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of
starting small. Cognition, 48, 71-99.
Hausser, R. (1999). Foundations of computational linguistics: Man-machine
communication in natural language. Berlin: Springer.
Hyams, N., & Wexler, K. (1993). On the grammatical basis of null subjects in child
language. Linguistic Inquiry, 24(3), 421-459.
MacWhinney, B. (1975). Pragmatic patterns in child syntax. Stanford Papers And
Reports on Child Language Development, 10, 153-165.
MacWhinney, B., & Leinbach, J. (1991). Implementations are not conceptualizations:
Revising the verb learning model. Cognition, 29, 121-157.
MacWhinney, B., Leinbach, J., Taraban, R., & McDonald, J. (1989). Language learning:
Cues or rules? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 255-277.
Meisel, J. (1986). Word order and case marking in early child language. Evidence from
simultaneous acquisition of two first languages: French and German. Linguistics,
Part 3: Morphosyntax 88

24, 123-185.
Mintz, T. H., Newport, E. L., & Bever, T. G. (2002). The distributional structure of
grammatical categories in speech to young children. Cognitive Science, 26, 393-
424.
Parisse, C., & Le Normand, M. T. (2000). Automatic disambiguation of the
morphosyntax in spoken language corpora. Behavior Research Methods,
Instruments, and Computers, 32, 468-481.
Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1991). U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a
multi-layered perceptron: Implications for child language acquisition. Cognition,
38, 43-102.
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., & Svartvik, J. (1985). A comprehensive grammar of
the English language. London: Longman.
Radford, A. (1990). Syntactic theory and the acquisition of English syntax. Oxford: Basil
Blackwell.
Rice, S. (1997). The analysis of ontogenetic trajectories: When a change in size or shape
is not heterochrony. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94, 907-
912.
Rose, Y., MacWhinney, B., Byrne, R., Hedlund, G., Maddocks, K., O’Brien, P., &
Wareham, T. (2005). Introducing Phon: A Software Solution for the Study of
Phonological Acquisition. In D. Bamman, T. Magnitskaia, & C. Zaller (Eds.),
30th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 489-
500). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Sagae, K., MacWhinney, B., & Lavie, A. (2004a). Adding syntactic annotations to
transcripts of parent-child dialogs. In LREC 2004 (pp. 1815-1818). Lisbon:
LREC.
Sagae, K., MacWhinney, B., & Lavie, A. (2004b). Automatic parsing of parent-child
interactions. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 113-
126.
Siskind, J. M. (1999). Learning word-to-meaning mappings. In J. Muure & P. Broeder
(Eds.), Cognitive Models of Language Acquisition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Stromswold, K. (1994). Using spontaneous production data to assess syntactic
development. In D. McDaniel, C. McKee, & H. Cairns (Eds.), Methods for
assessing children's syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a first language: A usage-based theory of language
acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
van Kampen, J. (1998). Left branch extraction as operator movement: Evidence from
child Dutch. In S. Powers & C. Hamman (Eds.), The acquisition of scrambling
and cliticization. Norwell: Kluwer.
Wexler, K. (1998). Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A
new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua, 106, 23-79.

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy