Slater Sample Chapter
Slater Sample Chapter
be approved by the
BRITISH
PSYCHOLOGICAL
SOCIETY
Edited by
An Introduction to
Developmental
Psychology
SECOND Edition
BPS TEXTBOOKS
An Introduction
to Developmental
Psychology
SECOND EDITION
EDITED BY ALAN SLATER AND
GAVIN BREMNER
Brief Contents
List of Contributors
Preface
Acknowledgements
Part I INTRODUCTION
1 The Scope and Methods of Developmental Psychology
Alan Slater, Scott P. Johnson, Darwin Muir
2 Theories and Issues in Child Development
Scott P. Johnson, Alan Slater, Ian Hocking
3 The Nature-Nurture Issue (An Illustration Using Behavior-Genetic
Research on Cognitive Development)
Elisa A. Esposito, Elena L. Grigorenko, Robert J. Sternberg
PART II INFANCY
4 Prenatal Development
Christine Moon and William P. Fifer
5 Perception, knowledge, and action in infancy
J. Gavin Bremner
6 Emotional development and attachment relationships
Elizabeth Meins
7 Social Interaction and the Beginnings of Communication
H. Rudolph Schaffer
8 The Development of Self and Gender
Wendy Lawrenson
9 Cognitive Development
Margaret Anne Defeyter
10 The Development of Language
Heather M. Hill, Stan A. Kuczaj II
11 Acquiring a Theory of Mind
Peter Mitchell
IV BRIEF CONTENTS
PART IV ADOLESCENCE
18 Educational implications
Alyson Davis & Naomi Winstone
19 Risk and Resilience in Development
Leslie Morrison Gutman and Eirini Flouri
20 Social problems in schools
Peter K Smith, Julian Elliott
21 Atypical Development and Risk factors
Sarah Norgate
Glossary
Index
2 Theories and Issues in Child
Development
SCOTT P. JOHNSON, ALAN SLATER & IAN HOCKING
KEY TERMS
accommodation animism assimilation behaviour genetics behaviourism bottom-up
structures castration complex centration cephalocaudal trend chromosomes classical
conditioning cognitive adaptations concrete operations stage connectionism conservation
tasks constructivism constructivist theory continuity versus discontinuity critical period
dynamic systems theory ego egocentric Electra complex ethological approaches formal
law of effect maturation mechanistic world view microgenetic studies monotropy motor
learning theory stability versus change strange situation strategies superego theory of
development top-down structures zone of proximal development
CHAPTER OUTLINE
OVERVIEW
This chapter sets the theoretical background for the material in the chapters to follow. The coverage
of theoretical approaches is broad, and will give the reader a good introduction to the diversity of
explanations of childrens development.
First, different theories of motor development are outlined, and the authors point to the advantages of
dynamic systems theory according to which motor development is a product of the interplay between
brain structure, the structure and dynamics of the body, and the structure of the environment.
Next, the chapter considers theories of cognitive development. Piagets stage theory is central here,
and receives a thorough treatment. His theory is contrasted with the information processing account.
Whereas Piagets theory treats early deficits in thought as due to lack of logical ability, information
processing accounts identify processing deficits as the problem, in particular, limitations in memory.
A large number of theoretical approaches stress the social environment in some way or other.Vygotskys
theory treats higher cognitive structures as coming from the social world, becoming internalised as a
result of interactions with knowledgeable others. Behaviourist theories are all based on the principle
that the social world, and in particular the parents, shape the behaviour of the individual, and the best
example of application of these accounts to child development is Albert Banduras social learning theory.
Other theories have their origins in evolutionary theory, and the best example in developmental
psychology is attachment theory, originally formulated by John Bowlby, according to which formation
of a secure emotional attachment between infant and caregiver is a vital prerequisite for emotional
stability. Attachment theory is closely related to psychoanalytic approaches, the prime example being
Freuds theory of psychosexual development, according to which emotional problems in adulthood can
be traced to problems the child encountered in one of the psychosexual stages. Humanistic theories
bear certain similarities to psychoanalytic theory. For instance, Maslows account proposes a hierarchy
of needs that humans must achieve to reach a satisfactory adult state.
The authors summarise the sections on theories by pointing out, through examples, the fact that
different theories are not necessarily mutually exclusive. Often, one theory explains some aspects of
behaviour, while another theory fills in more of the story.
The chapter ends by summarising some key issues that will reappear in the pages that follow, namely
the naturenurture issue, stability versus change, and continuity versus discontinuity in development.
Different theories very clearly say different things with respect to these distinctions, and the challenge for
developmental psychology is to weigh these different accounts against each other.
or . . .
INTRODUCTION
Human development is rich, varied, and enormously complex. We should not expect,
therefore, that any single theory of development will do justice to this complexity,
and indeed no theory attempts to do this. Each theory attempts to account for only a
limited range of development and it is often the case that within each area of devel-
opment there are competing theoretical views, each attempting to account for the
same aspects of development. We will see some of this complexity and conflict in our
account of different theoretical views, and in Chapter 1 we have seen that different
ways of studying children lead to different developmental functions, and these are
linked with different theoretical views.
Before beginning our account of theories of development it is helpful to say what
we mean by a theory, as this is a term that has many definitions.
theory of development
a scheme or system of
For our purposes a theory of development is a scheme or system
ideas that is based on of ideas that is based on evidence and attempts to explain, describe
evidence and attempts and predict behaviour and development. From this account it is clear
to explain, describe, and that a theory attempts to bring order to what might otherwise be a
predict behaviour and
development.
chaotic mass of information for this reason we can see why there
is nothing so practical as a good theory!
In every area of development there are at least two kinds of theory which we can
call the minor and the major. What we are calling minor theories are those which deal
only with very specific, narrow areas of development. So, for example, there are theo-
ries about the way in which eye movements develop, about the origins of pointing,
and so on. Major theories are those which attempt to explain large areas of develop-
ment, and it is these that are the focus of this chapter.
To make our account of theories more orderly and understandable, we have
divided them into six broad groups:
Motor development
Cognitive development
Social-cognitive development
Evolution and ethology
Psychoanalytic theories
Humanistic theory
MOTOR DEVELOPMENT
motor milestones the One of the most obvious signs of development in infancy is the baby
basic motor skills
acquired in infancy and
achieving the various motor milestones. Parents are very proud
early childhood, such as of these acquisitions and they are a focus of parental conversations
sitting unaided, standing, about their infants Billy can sit now, Helen has just started to
crawling, walking. crawl, Jimmy can walk without help, Rachel loves to climb up
THEORIES AND ISSUES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT 5
stairs. The development of motor skills has very important implications for other
aspects of development. The ability to act on the world affects all other aspects of
development, and each new accomplishment brings with it an increasing degree
of independence. For example, when infants begin to crawl they become independ-
ently mobile and one of the major transitions in early development begins. These
changes affect emotional and social development, communication, appreciation of
heights, and an understanding of distance and space (Campos et al., 2000).
Table 2.1 charts the sequence of development of various motor milestones dur-
ing infancy. At birth the infant has a number of well-developed motor skills, which
include sucking, looking, grasping, breathing, crying skills that are vital for survival.
However, the general impression of the newborn is one of uncoordinated inability
and general weakness. Movements of the limbs appear jerky and uncoordinated, and
it takes a few weeks before infants can lift their head from a prone position. The
muscles are clearly unable to support the babys weight in order to allow such basic
activities as sitting, rolling over, or standing. By the end of infancy, around 18 months,
all this has changed (Figure 2.1). The toddler can walk, run, climb, communicate in
speech and gesture, and use the two hands in complex coordinated actions.
The questions that a theory of motor development needs to explain include the
following: Do the early motor activities prepare the way for the more complex volun-
tary activities that follow, and if so, how do they do it? How do new motor patterns
(such as pointing, running, speaking, tool use) develop since they appear to be quali-
tatively different from earlier patterns? As we shall see, the answers to these questions
are complex.
If you look at Table 2.1 two things will become apparent. First is that the different
motor milestones emerge in a regular sequence sitting with support, sitting unaided,
crawling, standing, walking, and climbing appear almost always in this order. The
second is that there is a considerable age range in which individual infants achieve
each skill for example, some infants crawl at 5 months while others are as late as 11
months. These two aspects of motor development give separate support to the two
major theories of motor development that we will discuss here maturational theories
and dynamic systems theory.
Maturational theories
One of the first psychologists to investigate human motor develop- cephalocaudal trend
ment was Arnold Gesell, who studied hundreds of hours of films motor development that
of motor activity in longitudinal studies of children from birth to 9 proceeds in infancy from
head to foot along the
years (e.g., Gesell & Ames, 1940). He concluded that motor devel-
length of the body.
opment proceeded from the global to the specific in two directions.
One direction is called the cephalocaudal trend and is from head proximodistal trend
to foot along the length of the body that is, control of the head is the development of
motor control in infancy
first, then the arms and trunk, and finally control of the legs. The which is from the centre
other direction of development is what is called the proximodistal of the body outwards
trend, which is that motor control is from the centre of the body to more peripheral
outwards to more peripheral segments that is, the head, trunk, and segments.
6 AN INTRODUCTION TO DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
13 months Stepping reflex, lifts head, sits Grasps object if placed in hands, sucks,
with support. control of eye movements, the first smile.
24 months When prone lifts head and uses Grasps cube when placed near hand.
arms for support.
58 months Sits without support. Reaches for and grasps object, using one
hand.
510 months Stands with support, and pulls Points at object of interest, grasps with
self to stand. thumb and finger (pincer grip ).
1014 months Stands alone, and walks alone. Puts objects into small containers,
builds tower of cubes. Produces first
meaningful word.
1318 months Walks backwards and sideways, Holds crayon with fingers, scribbles
runs, climbs, walks up stairs. energetically.
1830 months Runs easily, jumps, skips, rides Vocabulary and articulation increases
and steers tricycle, walks on rapidly, picks up small objects (e.g.
tiptoe. candy/sweets).
THEORIES AND ISSUES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT 7
microgenetic studies forgo the crawling stage entirely, and after several months of sitting
studies of development and shuffling may stand and then walk (Adolph & Joh, 2007). In addi-
that observe individual tion to these observations there are what are called microgenetic
children in great detail
from the time they first studies of motor development in which experimenters observe indi-
attempt a new skill, suchvidual infants or children from the time they first attempt a new skill,
as walking or crawling, such as walking or crawling, until it is performed effortlessly (e.g.,
until it is performed Gill, Adolph & Vereijken, 2009). From these studies it becomes clear
effortlessly.
that infants acquisition of a new motor skill is much the same as that
of adults learning a new motor skill the beginnings are usually fumbling and poor,
there is trial and error learning and great concentration, all gradually leading to the
accomplished skilful activity, which then is usually used in the development of yet
new motor skills.
According to the dynamic systems theory all new motor development is the result
of a dynamic and continual interaction of three major factors: (1) nervous system
development; (2) the capabilities and biomechanics of the body; (3) environmental
constraints and support (Thelen & Spencer, 1998). We can illustrate this dynamic
interplay by considering three separate studies on infant kicking, reaching and sitting,
and walking.
Infant kicking
Esther Thelen (1999) tested 24 three-month-olds on a foot-kicking task in which each
infant was placed in a crib in a supine (lying on their back) position and a soft elastic
ankle cuff was attached to one leg, and the cuff, in turn, was attached by a cord to
an overhead brightly coloured mobile. By kicking the leg the babies could make the
mobile dance around and they quickly learned to make this exciting event happen.
In this condition the other leg the one that was not connected to the mobile move-
ments either moved independently or alternately with the attached leg (Figure 2.2).
Then Thelen changed the arrangement by yoking the legs together. She did this by
putting ankle cuffs on both legs, and joining the two together with a strip of Velcro.
What happened then was that the infants initially tried to kick the legs separately
because moving the legs alternately is the more natural action but gradually learned
to kick both together to get the mobile to move.
This study shows that the infants were able to change their pattern of interlimb
coordination to solve a novel, experimentally imposed task.
Infant reaching
Thelen and Spencer (1998) followed the same four infants from 3 weeks to 1 year (a
longitudinal study) in order to explore the development of successful reaching. Their
aim was to look at the interrelationship between different motor systems. What they
found was that infants acquired stable control over the head several weeks before
the onset of reaching, then there was a reorganisation of muscle patterns so that the
infants could stabilise the head and shoulder. These developments gave the infants a
stable base from which to reach, and successful reaching followed. This is an indica-
tion that infants need a stable posture before they can attain the goal of reaching
successfully, and is a clear demonstration that new motor skills are learned through a
process of modifying and developing their already existing abilities.
THEORIES AND ISSUES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT 9
FIGURE 2.2 The ankle ribbon attached to the babys foot causes the mobile to jiggle about
when she kicks her legs
This is an example of operant conditioning, the infant learns the contingency between kicking and
reward
Source: Carolyn Rovee-Collier, reprinted with permission.
Infant walking
Newborn infants are extremely top heavy, with big heads and weak legs. Over the com-
ing years their body weight is gradually redistributed and their centre of mass gradu-
ally moves downwards until it finishes slightly above the navel. Adolph and Avolio
(2000, p. 1148) put it rather nicely It is as if infants bodies are growing to fit their
comparatively large heads! This means that as infants and children grow they need
constantly to adjust and adapt their motor activities to accommodate the naturally
occurring changes to their body dimensions. There can be few clearer demonstrations
that the motor system is dynamic and constantly changing than this simple fact.
Adolph and Avolio give a nice demonstration of the way in which infants can
make adjustments over a very short period of time. They tested 14-month-olds by
having them wear saddlebags slung over each shoulder. The saddle bags increased the
infants chest circumference by the same amount in each of two conditions: feather-
weight filled with pillow-stuffing, weighing the negligible amount of 120 g and
lead-weight the not so negligible amount of between 2.2 and 3.0 kg, which increased
their body weight by 25 per cent and raised their centre of mass (raising the centre
of mass leads to increased instability, and is similar to a backpacker carrying a heavy
pack). They found that the lead-weight infants were more cautious, and made pro-
longed exploratory movements swaying, touching and leaning before attempt-
ing to walk down a slope. That is, these infants were testing their new-found body
dimensions and weight, and adjusted their judgements of what they could and could
10 AN INTRODUCTION TO DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
not do. These findings again demonstrate that infants do not have a fixed and rigid
understanding of their own abilities, and have the dynamic flexibility to adjust their
abilities as they approach each novel motor problem.
Overview
Despite the apparent appeal of maturational theories of motor development, research
over the last 20 years has demonstrated that motor skills are learned, both during
infancy and throughout life. The apparently invariant ordering of the motor mile-
stones is partly dictated by logical necessity you cant run before you can walk! and
is not necessarily invariant (you can walk before you can crawl!) From a consideration
of the studies described above it becomes clear that motor development cannot be
accounted for by any maturational theory. These and other findings contribute to the
emerging view of infants as active participants in their own motor-skill acquisition,
in which developmental change is empowered through infants everyday problem-
solving activities (Thelen, 1999, p. 103).
The emphasis on children as active participants in their own development is an
essential characteristic of the theoretical views offered by The Giant of Developmental
Psychology, Jean Piaget, whose claim was that childrens ability to act on the world
underlies their cognitive development, and we now turn our attention to his views.
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
Piagets theory of development
Everyone knows that Piaget was the most important figure the field has ever known . . . [he]
transformed the field of developmental psychology.
(Flavell, 1996, p. 200)
Once psychologists looked at development through Piagets eyes, they never saw children
in quite the same way.
(Miller, 1993, p. 81)
accommodation the mouth to use the familiar activity of sucking; the child meets a new
cognitive process teacher and treats them in the same way they treat teachers.
through which children
Accommodation is where individuals have to modify or change
adapt to new experi-
ences by modifying their their schemas, or ways of thinking, in order to adjust to a new situation.
preexisting schemas. An For example: infants might be presented with a toy that is larger than
important process in those they have previously handled, and so will have to adjust their
Piagets theory.
fingers and grasp to hold it; when children meet a new teacher who
schemas mental is different from their previous teachers they have to adjust their
structures in the childs
thinking that provide
way of thinking to understand the new person. It is worth stressing
representations and that assimilation and accommodation always occur together during
plans for enacting infancy and the examples given above are both cases of assimilation
behaviours. and accommodation occurring together.
functional invariants Throughout life the processes of assimilation and accommoda-
processes that do not tion are always active as we constantly strive to adapt to the world
change during develop-
ment, such as accommo-
we encounter. These processes, therefore, are what can be called
dation and assimilation functional invariants in that they dont change during development.
in Piagets theory. What do change are the cognitive structures (often called schemes)
sensorimotor stage in that allow the child to comprehend the world at progressively higher
Piagets theory, the first levels of understanding. According to Piagets view there are differ-
stage of cognitive develop- ent levels of cognitive understanding that take the child from the
ment, whereby thought
is based primarily on
activity-based sensorimotor functioning in infancy to the abstract
action and internalized levels of thought found in adolescence.
thinking is largely absent.
This stage is characteristic
of infants from birth to
about 2 years old.
The four stages of cognitive development
preoperational stage a Children move through four broad stages of development, each of
stage of development which is characterised by qualitatively different ways of thinking
described by Piaget in (Piaget, 1962). These stages are the sensorimotor stage of infancy,
which children under the
age of approximately 7 the preoperational stage of early childhood, the concrete opera-
years are unable to see tions stage of middle childhood, and the formal operations stage
enough aspects of prob- of adolescence and beyond. We will give a brief account of each of
lems in order to solve these stages, together with the approximate ages at which they are
them.
found note that these ages are only approximate and individual chil-
concrete operations drens development will often be slower or quicker.
stage the third Piagetian
stage of development in
which reasoning is said Sensorimotor stage (birth to 2 years)
to become more logical, This is one of the most impressive and dramatic areas of develop-
systematic, and rational
in its application to con-
ment. The child changes from the helpless newborn to the think-
crete objects. ing and knowing toddler, that is, to the cognitive individual with a
mind. These changes take place as a result of the infants actions
formal operations
stage the fourth on the objects and people in its environments, and this stage is the
Piagetian stage in which development of thought in action. As a result, infants learn to solve
the individual acquires problems, such as pulling a cloth to obtain an out-of-reach toy, and
the capacity for abstract they learn that objects continue to exist even though they cannot be
scientific thought. This
includes the ability to seen or heard. As the stage draws to a close the infant, now a tod-
theorise about impossi- dler whose language is developing rapidly, is able to reason through
ble events and items. thought as well as through sensorimotor activities.
THEORIES AND ISSUES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT 13
Overview
In Piagets theory we have a comprehensive and detailed account of cognitive devel-
opment from birth to adulthood. Cognitive development proceeds through a series
of stages, each more complex than the last, and each building on the achievements of
the previous one. In many respects, aspects of Piagets theory seem obvious
of course children are active in shaping their own development. But it was many
years before his theories began to make an impact on American and British psychol-
ogy. This was primarily due to three factors. First, American and British psychology
was dominated by the theoretical school of thought known as behav-
mechanistic world
iourism, which offered the mechanistic world view that the child is
view the idea that a per-
son can be represented inherently passive until stimulated by the environment and so the
as being like a machine opposing view offered by Piaget took time to be accepted. Second,
(such as a computer), Piaget only ever wrote in French, which made his work less acces-
which is inherently pas-
sible to English-speaking psychologists. And, third, while Piaget was
sive until stimulated by
the environment. a brilliant thinker, his writings are often extraordinarily complex and
difficult to understand!
Piagets full impact awaited the arrival of one man who could summarise, syn-
thesise and present his theoretical views in a way that was comprehensible and avail-
able to the English-speaking world. This was John H. Flavell whose The Developmental
Psychology of Jean Piaget appeared in 1963 (and in the foreword to this book Piaget
wrote I am not an easy author; hence it must have required an immense effort at
comprehension and intellectual empathy to have produced the clear and straightfor-
ward presentation that is found here). More recently, Flavell (1996) wrote an assess-
ment of Piagets contribution, entitled Piagets legacy and quotes an anonymous
reviewer of his article The impact of Piaget on developmental psychology is . . . too
monumental to embrace and at the same time too omnipresent to detect, to which
Flavell simply adds the words I agree.
INFORMATION PROCESSING
APPROACHES
Information processing approaches view the human mind as a complex system
through which information flows. Information processing accounts of human cog-
nition include current views of memory formation, with terms such as encoding,
THEORIES AND ISSUES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT 15
storage, retrieval, strategies, metamemory, and this account is given in information process-
Chapter 13. A brief account of information processing explanations ing the view that
of cognitive changes in adolescence is given in Chapter 16. cognitive processes are
explained in terms of
Information processing theories are rooted in three 20th-century inputs and outputs and
innovations. The first is the rapid and continuing advances in compu- that the human mind is
ter technology. The second is the view, revolutionary at the time, that a system through which
an organisms behaviour cannot be understood without knowing the information flows.
structure of the perceivers environmentfor example, the structure
of light reflected from objects (Gibson, 1979). The third is construc- strategies knowledge
built up to solve particu-
tivism, a theory about how perception fills in information that can- lar problems.
not be seen or heard directly, such as inferring the parts of an object
that are hidden from view via processes of inference. Piagets theory constructivism Piagets
is also regarded as a constructivist view, because the child is pro- theoretical view that
posed to construct their knowledge from existing perceptual and cog- infants are not born with
nitive skills. Gibsons theory is also a constructive view, but primarily knowledge about the
world, but instead gradu-
concerned with how we construct our perceptual, rather than cogni- ally construct knowledge
tive, world. Gibsons theory and constructivism were opposed to a and the ability to repre-
theoretical viewpoint that was dominant at mid-century, behaviour- sent reality mentally.
ism, whose principal tenet was that our knowledge of an organism
is limited exclusively to what we can observe, and a position that avoided discussions
of what goes on inside the mind. Investigations of both perception and cognition and
their development, therefore, were severely constrained and, ultimately, unsatisfying
hence the need for new theoretical approaches.
Information processing theories, therefore, focus on the information available in
the external environment, and the means by which the child receives and interprets
this information. This way of thinking can provide clarity with respect to understand-
ing many aspects of cognitive development. The task of the developing child is to use
their perceptual systems vision, hearing, touch, and so forth to explore the world
and obtain information about its properties. The information must be attended to,
encoded, stored, retrieved, and acted upon to build knowledge of objects and their
characteristics.
In contrast to Piagets theory, information processing theories
are not the product of one persons work, but instead represent a bottom-up a cognitive
development process
number of scientists working with a common set of assumptions. In beginning with the
the following sections we provide some recent examples. input or uptake of infor-
mation by the child, and
building complex sys-
tems of knowledge from
Cognitive development in infancy simpler origins.
experience and thus come to solve problems more quickly and accurately. A more
detailed account of Sieglers view of development is given in Chapter 9.
growth, genes and gene expression, and the development of circuits and networks in
the brain, and as such epitomise both a major theoretical advancement and a more
realistic representation of human development (Westermann et al., 2007).
Summary
The theories and research we have described are motivated by multiple notions
of information: the information available in the stimulus, the uptake of that infor-
mation, the processing of the information by the individual, and the individuals
response. Understanding of information at these different levels is a central task of
information processing theory, and multiple methods are used in pursuit of this goal:
empirical studies of infants and children (including close observations of behaviours
at a microgenetic scale), connectionist models, and recordings of brain activity.
SOCIAL-COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
Whereas Piaget tended to focus on the individual child attempting to make sense of
the world (given some basic tools) other researchers have been interested in the inter-
action between the child and their community the social environment.
Vygotsky
Born in the same year as Piaget, the Russian psychologist Lev Semenovich Vygotsky
(18961934) was one of the first to recognise the importance of knowledgeable
adults in the childs environment. For him, the development of intellectual abilities
is influenced by a didactic relationship (one based on instructive dialogue) with more
advanced individuals. One fascinating facet of his work is the claim that higher men-
tal abilities are first encountered and used competently in social interactions, only
later being internalised and possessed as individual thought processes. For instance,
language is used socially to quite a level of competence before it is internalised, reor-
ganising thought in the process.
Thus, a major theme in Vygotskys theories is that social interaction plays a funda-
mental role in cognitive development. He argued that there is a gap between what the
child knows and what they can be taught. At a given stage of devel- zone of proximal devel-
opment the child has a certain level of understanding, a temporary opment (ZPD) the dif-
maximum. A little beyond this point lies the zone of proximal devel- ference between what
opment (ZPD). This zone can be seen as representing problems and children can do on their
own, and what they can
ideas that are just a little too difficult for the child to understand on do under adult guidance
their own. It can, however, be explored and understood with the help or in collaboration with
of an adult. Thus the adult can guide the child because they have a more able peers.
firmer grasp of the more complex thinking involved. introspectionism an
Vygotsky died young (from tuberculosis) but he left an impressive approach to psychology
amount of work (over 100 published articles and books) which con- common in the nine-
teenth century in which
tinues to have an impact on developmental psychology. A compari- observers were asked to
son of Vygotskys views with those of Piaget is given in Chapter 9, reflect on their thoughts,
and his contribution to education is discussed in detail in Chapter 19. feelings, and perceptions.
classical conditioning
a method of learning
first investigated by the
Behaviourism and social learning theory Russian physiologist Ivan
Pavlov in the early part
Early behaviourism of the 20th century. In
Towards the end of the nineteenth century, psychology experienced this form of conditioning,
a swing away from the subjective perspective of introspectionism certain behaviours can
(the analysis of self-reported perceptions) towards a more objective be elicited by a neutral
(normally unstimulat-
method. This scientific approach to psychology had its roots in the ing) stimulus because of
work of Vygotskys countryman, Ivan Petrovich Pavlov (18491936). its learned association
Pavlov developed a grand theory of learning called classical condi- with a more powerful
tioning. According to this theory, certain behaviours can be elicited stimulus.
20 AN INTRODUCTION TO DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
operate (emit behaviours) on their environments. It is still the case that the childs
development is dominated by their environment, but Skinners viewpoint allowed
for more flexible and generative patterns of behaviour. According to Skinners view
it is possible to shape the animals or childs behaviour by manipulating the reinforce-
ment received.
We can see the role of reinforcement in this brief account of infant behaviour
(Skinner, 1961, p. 418):
One reinforcer to which babies often respond is the flashing on and off of a table lamp.
Whenever the baby lifts its hand, flash the light. In a short time a well-defined response
will be generated. (Human babies are just as smart as dogs or pigeons in this respect.)
Incidentally, the baby will enjoy the experience.
It is certainly the case that our behaviour is guided by reward and punishment, and
behaviourism continues to be used in the control of behaviour. Skinner gave an
account of how parents may unwittingly promote undesirable behaviours, such as
aggression, crying or shouting in their children. If, for example, the mother only gives
the child attention when it is misbehaving, then her positive reinforcement of atten-
tion is likely to promote the very behaviour she does not want! The remedy is this
(Skinner, 1961, p. 419):
The remedy in such a case is simply for the mother to make sure that she responds with
attention and affection to most if not all the responses of the child which are . . . acceptable . . .
and that she never reinforces the annoying forms of behavior.
social learning theory
Social learning theory associated with Albert
Whereas behaviourism had important but rather vague things to Bandura. The application
of behaviourism to social
say about the childs acquisition of behaviour patterns, the work and cognitive learning
of Albert Bandura (1925) examined particular behaviours in more that emphasises the
detail. His behaviourism was less mechanistic than that of Skinner. importance of observa-
He did not focus only on observable behaviour, but posited process- tional learning, that is,
learning by observation
ing that occurred within the mind a construct specifically denied by and then copying (imi-
his behaviourist colleagues. His approach was initially named sociobe- tating) the observed acts.
haviourism, then social learning theory.
During the 1960s Bandura carried out a series of experiments on childhood
aggression. In one, some children were divided into two groups. The first (control)
group saw an adult playing with toys, one of which was an inflatable Bobo doll. The
second (experimental) group saw the same adult, this time playing aggressively with
the toys, hitting the doll with a hammer. When allowed to play individually, Bandura
observed that children from the experimental group behaved in a
more aggressive way towards their own Bobo doll. observational learning
So, without obvious reinforcement, a particular aggressive behav- situation in which peo-
iour had been learned. Bandura termed this observational learn- ple (especially children)
learn by observing oth-
ing or vicarious conditioning. In some sense, the child had mentally ers and then copying
assumed the role of the observed person and taken note of any rein- (imitating) the observed
forcement. Bandura concluded that children imitate the actions of acts.
22 AN INTRODUCTION TO DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
FIGURE 2.3 Newly hatched chicks will follow their mother and siblings
Source: Jan de Wild/Shutterstock.
24 AN INTRODUCTION TO DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
Attachment theory John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth
Mother love in infancy and childhood is as important for mental health as are vitamins and
proteins for physical health.
(Bowlby, 1952)
The British physician and psychoanalyst John Bowlby was inspired by observa-
tions of imprinting, and was one of the first to offer an ethological and evolutionary
interpretation of human development. The concluding comment to his 1952 book
(given above) is one of the most widely quoted within developmental psychology. His
contribution to our understanding of attachment formation in infancy and childhood
continues to have an immense impact, and here we will give a very brief account of
his views, and that of his American colleague Mary Ainsworth.
Prior to Bowlby the prevailing belief, stemming from Behaviourism
primary drives basic
needs which include is that the attachment of infants to their caregivers was a secondary
hunger, thirst, and the drive, that is, because the mother (or primary caregiver) satisfies
need for warmth. Bowlby the babys primary drives (these include hunger, thirst and the need
and others have argued for warmth) she acquires secondary reinforcing properties. However,
that an infants need
for attachment is also a Bowlby pointed out that the need for attachment was itself a primary
primary drive. drive (as the quote given above indicates, which is the conclusion to
his 1952 report to the World Health Organization).
Several lines of evidence have since supported this conclusion. In the 1950s and
1960s Harry Harlow and his colleagues (e.g. Harlow & Zimmerman, 1959) separated
baby monkeys from their real mothers and offered them two surrogate (substitute)
mothers. One of these was made of wire, but had a nipple attached which provided
food (and hence satisfied the primary drives of hunger and thirst). The other was
made of soft cloth and provided no nutrition. What they found is that the baby mon-
keys fed from the wire mother, but cuddled up to the soft cloth mother, and ran
THEORIES AND ISSUES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT 25
Using the strange situation Ainsworth discovered that there are several attachment
styles that differ in degree of security. A detailed account of these attachment styles
and of Bowlbys and Ainsworths contribution in developing what is called attachment
theory is given in detail in Chapter 6. For the moment we can conclude that their
importance has been in demonstrating the importance of early secure attachments
and showing that these attachments are as basic and important as any other human
drive or motivation.
psychoanalytic theory
most prominently asso-
PSYCHOANALYTIC THEORIES
ciated with Sigmund
Freud. Freud suggested
that there are three main Sigmund Freud The founder of psychoanalysis
personality structures:
the ego, the id and the For generations almost every branch of human knowledge will be enriched
superego.
and illuminated by the imagination of Freud.
id in Freuds theory, a (Jane Harrison, 18501928)
primitive collection of
urges with which an
His place is not, as he claimed, with Copernicus and Darwin, but with Hans
individual begins life.
The id is responsible for Christian Anderson and the Brother Grimm, tellers of fairy tales.
an individuals primitive (Hans Eysenck, 191697)
instincts, such as eating
and reproducing.
As will be apparent from the above, not everyone agrees that
ego in Freuds theory, Freuds contribution to knowledge has been entirely positive! Freud
the ego can be thought claimed that much of our behaviour is determined by unconscious
of as the rational
thought that evolved forces of which we are not directly aware. In presenting his psycho-
to control the urges of analytic theory he suggested that there are three main structures to
the id in order that they personality: the id, the ego, and the superego. The id is present in
meet the demands of the newborn infant and consists of impulses, emotions and desires.
reality and maintain
social approval and It demands instant gratification of all its wishes and needs. As this is
esteem. impractical the ego develops to act as a practical interface or media-
superego in Freuds
tor between reality and the desires of the id. The final structure to
theory, a collection of develop is the superego, which is the sense of duty and responsibility
ideals, an individuals in many ways the conscience.
morality. This is what The ego and the superego develop as the individual progresses
we refer to as our con-
science and it is often in
through the five psychosexual stages oral, anal, phallic, latency
conflict with our id. and genital and these are described next.
psychosexual stages
Freud argued that there
were five stages of
human development:
The five psychosexual stages
oral (01 year), anal (13
years), phallic (35 years),
Oral stage (approximately birth to 1 year)
latency (5 years The infants greatest satisfaction is derived from stimulation of the
adolescence), and genital lips, tongue, and mouth. Sucking is the chief source of pleasure for
(adolescence onwards). the young infant.
THEORIES AND ISSUES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT 27
is impossible to predict the childs development despite the fact that the first 6 years
from birth are supposedly critical in determining later personality formation.
FIGURE 2.5 The psychoanalyst tries to discover what it is that went wrong in your childhood
that is causing your current problems
Source: Blaj Gabriel/Shutterstock.
THEORIES AND ISSUES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT 29
hierarchy of needs
Abraham Maslows hierarchy of needs stages of needs or
desires in Abraham
Maslow suggested that there is a hierarchy of needs or motives that Maslows humanistic
determine our behaviour. The hierarchy is given below (Figure 2.6) theory which go from
and extends from the basic needs for survival through the search the basic physiological
needs for food and water
for self-actualisation. To see how Maslows hierarchy might work, to the ultimate desire
imagine the following scenario (based on Dworetsky, 1995, p. 43. You for self-actualisation or
are a young man or woman who arrives as an emigrant/immigrant the desire to fulfill ones
to a foreign country, broke and homeless. Your first aim would be potential.
30 AN INTRODUCTION TO DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
Gender development
Gender development concerns the important question of how it is that children grow
up knowing that they are either a boy or a girl. Psychologists from several different
theoretical traditions have offered accounts of how this happens, and here we give
very brief accounts of cognitive, behaviourist/social learning, psychoanalytic and
biological explanations.
A cognitive account
A cognitive view of gender development was offered nearly 40 years ago by the
American psychologist Lawrence Kohlberg (1966, see also Chapter 14 for his theory
of the development of moral reasoning). According to Kohlbergs account of gen-
der development the child gradually comes to realise that she/he is a girl or a boy
and that this is unchangeable once a girl (or boy) always a girl (or boy), a realisa-
tion that is known as gender constancy. Most children come to this
gender constancy the
realisation some time after 3 years, and almost all know it by age 7 awareness, in early child-
(Tobin et al., 2010; Wehren & De Lisi, 1983). Kohlbergs theory sug- hood, that one is either
gests that once children understand which gender they are they will a boy or a girl, and that
develop appropriate gender-role behaviours. That is, knowing you this is unchangeable
once a girl (boy), always a
are a girl or a boy helps the child to organise their behaviour to be girl (boy).
gender-appropriate.
A psychoanalytic view
In the Freudian version of psychoanalytic theory a girls identification with her
mother, and a boys with his father, develop from the resolution of the Electra
and Oedipus complexes, as described above. As a result of this identification girls
32 AN INTRODUCTION TO DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
and boys form female and male identities (respectively!) and take on their same-
gender parents views and behaviour as their own.
Biological determinants
The accounts described so far all emphasise the role of nurture in promoting gen-
der development. But remember that the physical aspects of gender are biologically
determined by the type of chromosomes we inherit at conception (see Chapter 4).
Here we will describe a case history (which is described in more detail in chapter 8) to
highlight the role of nature (genetic and biological) in gender determination.
Bruce Reimer was one of two identical twins born on 22 August 1965. The twins
developed urinary problems and at 8 months Bruce (and his twin brother Brian)
were taken to a clinic for circumcision. What happened to Bruce was that he had his
penis accidentally burned to ablation during phimosis repair by cautery (Diamond &
Sigmundson, 1999, p. 58). What this medical terminology means is that the incompe-
tent physician destroyed Bruces penis.
At the time one of the most influential views on gender development was
expressed by the psychologist John Money (e.g. Money & Ehrhardt, 1972), and
was that individuals are psychosexually and gender neutral at birth, and that experi-
ence (nurture) is the sole determinant of their development. A decision was therefore
made to carry out gender-reassignment surgery (to create a vagina and female genital
appearance), and to rear Bruce as Brenda. This case is described in earlier textbooks
on child development as clear evidence that nurture determined gender roles, and
Moneys theoretical views achieved widespread acceptance, even to the point that
some were arguing that if a genetic male had a small penis (in extreme instances this
is referred to as a micropenis) then Often it is wiser to rear a genetic male as a female
(Donahoe & Hendren, 1976, p. 396).
But it all went drastically wrong. Even soon after the operation Brenda began
rejecting girl things, like refusing to wear dresses. Somewhere between the ages of
9 and 11 Brenda figured that I was a guy. At school she persisted in standing up to
urinate in the girls bathroom. She made several suicide attempts and finally learned
the truth. At this time Brenda refused to carry on as a female and insisted on gen-
der reassignment (which included a mastectomy and phallus reconstruction) to his
biologically determined gender, and called himself David. He later married an older
woman and adopted her children. This case is one that gives strong support for the
view that prenatal and early-infantile hormones have a strong influence on gender
development and other gender dimorphism characteristics.
Overview
These different accounts of gender development all have their appeal. It is clear that
social influences and childrens cognitive awareness influence their gender-related
behaviour. But it is also clear that biological (genetic/hormonal) influences are
important. Many, perhaps most, transsexuals (those who elect for gender reassign-
ment, often through surgical procedures) will say that they have felt that they were
a girl in a boys body (or vice versa) for as long as they can remember, even though
they may never been reinforced for gender-inappropriate behaviour. What is clear
is that we have different theoretical views and there are multiple causes of gender
THEORIES AND ISSUES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT 33
development in children. Perhaps biological factors provide the basic differences, and
cognitive and social factors add the fine detail to create behavioural differences.
It is often claimed that the child is father to the man (or the child is mother to the
woman), meaning that early experiences influence current and later development.
This view suggests that certain aspects of childrens development display stability, in
the sense that they are consistent and predictable across time. It turns out that devel-
opment is characterised by both stability and change for example, personality char-
acteristics such as shyness, and the tendency to be aggressive tend to be stable, while
others such as approach (the tendency to extreme friendliness and lack of caution with
strangers) and sluggishness (reacting passively to changing circumstances) are unstable
(as discussed in Chapter 15).
34 AN INTRODUCTION TO DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
DISCUSSION POINTS
1. Considering the evidence presented here, list as many aspects of motor development (a) that
may not depend on experience, (b) that probably do depend on experience.
2. Discuss ways in which Piagets account of development differs from (a) maturational accounts,
and (b) accounts that portray development as moulded by the environment.
THEORIES AND ISSUES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT 35
Bremner, G., & Slater, A. (2004). Theories of infant development. Oxford and Massachusetts: Blackwell.
Fancher, R.E. (1990). Pioneers of psychology (2nd edn). New York & London: W.W. Norton.
Green, M., & Piel, J.A. (2002). Theories of human development: a comparative approach. Massachusetts:
Allyn and Bacon.
Harris, M. (2008). Exploring developmental psychology: understanding theory and methods. Washington,
DC & London: Sage.
Thomas, R.M. (2000). Recent theories of human development. Washington, DC & London: Sage.
Thomas, R.M. (2005). Comparing theories of child development. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishers.
REFERENCES
Adolph, K.E., & Avolio, A.M. (2000). Walking infants adapt locomotion to changing body dimen-
sions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 11481166.
Adolph, K.E., & Joh, A.S. (2007). Motor development: how infants get in the act. In A. Slater &
M. Lewis (Eds.), Introduction to infant development (pp.6380). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Amso, D., & Johnson, S.P. (2006). Learning by selection: Visual search and object perception in
young infants. Developmental Psychology, 6, 12361245.
Bandura, A. (2001). Social Cognitive Theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review of Psychology,
52, 126.
Bandura, A. (2009). Social cognitive theory goes global. The Psychologist, 22, 504506.
Bliss, J. (2010). Recollections of Jean Piaget. The Psychologist, 23, 444446.
Bowlby, J. (1952). Maternal care and mental health. Geneva: World Health Organisation.
36 AN INTRODUCTION TO DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
Campos, J.J., Anderson, D.I., Barbu-Roth, M.A., Hubbard, E.M., Hertenstein, M.J., & Witherington,
D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149219.
Cohen, L.B., & Amsel, G. (1998). Precursors to infants perception of causality. Infant Behavior &
Development, 21, 713731.
Cohen, L.B., Chaput, H.H., & Cashon, C.H. (2002). A constructivist model of infant cognition.
Cognitive Development, 17, 13231343.
Diamond, M., & Sigmundson, H.K. (1999). Sex reassignment at birth. In S.J. Ceci & W.M. Williams
(Eds.), The naturenurture debate: The essential readings (pp.5580). Oxford and Massachusetts:
Blackwell.
Donahoe, P.K., & Hendren, W.H.I. (1976), Evaluation of the newborn with ambiguous genitalia.
Pediatric Clinics of North America, 23, 361370.
Dworetsky, J.P. (1995). Human development: A lifespan approach. New York: West Publishing.
Flavell, J.H. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. Toronto, New York and London: Van
Nostrand.
Flavell, J.H. (1996). Piagets legacy. Psychological Science, 7, 200203.
Gesell, A., & Ames, L. (1940). The ontogenetic organization of prone behavior in human infancy.
Journal of Genetic Psychology, 56, 247263.
Gibson, J.J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gill, S.V., Adolph, K.E., & Vereijken, B. (2009). Change in action: How infants learn to walk down
slopes. Developmental Science, 12, 888902.
Gottlieb, J.P., Kusunoki, M., & Goldberg, M.E. (1998). The representation of visual salience in
monkey parietal cortex. Nature, 391, 481484.
Harlow, H., & Zimmerman, R.R. (1959). Affectional responses in the infant monkey. Science, 130,
421432.
Johnson, S.P. (2004). Development of perceptual completion in infancy. Psychological Science, 15,
769775.
Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of childrens sex-role concepts and atti-
tudes. In E.E. Macoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 81173). Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Leslie, A.M. (1984). Spatiotemporal continuity and the perception of causality in infants. Perception,
13, 287305.
Lewin, K. (1944). The dynamics of group action. Educational Leadership, 1, 195200.
McGraw, M. (1945). Neuromuscular maturation of the human infant. New York: Hafner.
Money, J., & Ehrhardt, A.A. (1972). Man and woman/boy and girl. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
University Press.
Miller, P.H. (1993). Theories of developmental psychology (3rd edn). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Piaget, J. (1960). The childs conception of the world. London: Routledge.
Piaget, J. (1962). The stages of the intellectual development of the child. Bulletin of the Mennigner
Clinic, 26, 120128. Reprinted in A. Slater & D. Muir (1999). (Eds.), The Blackwell reader in devel-
opmental psychology, pp.3542. Oxford and Massachusetts: Blackwell.
Schaffer, H.R., & Emerson, P.E. (1964). The development of social attachments in infancy. Monographs
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 29, No 94.
Schlesinger, M., Amso, D., & Johnson, S.P. (2007). The neural basis for visual selective attention in
young infants: A computational account. Adaptive Behavior, 15, 135148.
Siegler, R.S., & Shipley, C. (1995). Variation, selection, and cognitive change. In T. Simon & G.
Halford (Eds.), Developing cognitive competence: New approaches to process modeling (pp.3176).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Skinner, B.F. (1961). Cumulative record. London: Methuen.
THEORIES AND ISSUES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT 37
Sternberg, R.J. & Okagaki, L. (1989). Continuity and discontinuity in intellectual development are
not a matter of eitheror. Human Development, 32, 158166.
Thelen, E. (1999). Three-month-old infants can learn task-specific patterns of interlimb coordina-
tion. In K. Lee (Ed.), Childhood cognitive development: the essential readings (pp. 91105). Oxford:
Blackwell.
Thelen, E., & Spencer, J.P. (1998). Postural control during reaching in young infants: A dynamic
systems approach. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 22, 507514.
Tobin, D.D., Menon, M., Menon, M., Spatta, B.C., Hodges, E.V.E., & Perry, D.G. (2010). The intra-
psychics of gender: A model of self-socialization. Psychological Review, 117, 601622.
Watson, J.B. (1970). Behaviorism. New York: Norton.
Wehren, A., & De Lisi, R. (1983). The development of gender understanding: judgements and
explanations. Child Development, 54, 15681578.
Westermann, G., Mareschal, D., Johnson, M.H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M.W., & Thomas, M.S.C.
(2007). Neuroconstructivism. Developmental Science, 10, 7583.